Order for this Paper or similar Assignment Help Service

Fill the order form in 3 easy steps - Less than 5 mins.

Posted: February 14th, 2023

2014 Occupational knowledge in college students: Examining relations to career certainty, career decision-making self-efficacy, and interest congruence

Capstones, Theses and
Dissertations
2014
Occupational knowledge in college students:
Examining relations to career certainty, career
decision-making self-efficacy, and interest
congruence

iii
iv
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS v
ABSTRACT vi
CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION 1
CHAPTER 2. LITERATURE REVIEW 9
CHAPTER 3. METHODS 50
CHAPTER 4. RESULTS 58
CHAPTER 5. DISCUSSION 70
REFERENCES
APPENDICES
Demographics Questionnaire…………………………………
Work Activity Importance Profile……………………………
Occupation Work Activity Importance Profile ………………
Perceived Occupational Knowledge Scale……………….…..
Career Certainty Scale………………………………………..
Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale…………………………
Career Exploration Checklist…………………………………
O*NET Interest Profiler – Short Form……………………….
81
91
92
96
97
98
99
101
102
iii
TABLES
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics and Variable Correlations……………………………… 60
Table 2. Multiple Regression Results for Mediation Hypotheses 1 – 4………………… 62
Table 3. Standardized Path Coefficients for the Recursive Model Predicting
Career Certainty……………………………………………………………….. 68
iv
FIGURES
Figure 1. The hypothesized recursive path model predicting career certainty.……….… 8
Figure 2. Hypothesis 1 mediation model with multiple regression results……………… 63
Figure 3. Hypothesis 2 mediation model with multiple regression results……………… 64
Figure 4. Hypothesis 3 mediation model with multiple regression results……………… 65
Figure 5. Hypothesis 4 mediation model with multiple regression results……………… 66
Figure 7. Recursive path model predicting career certainty…………………………….. 69
v
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Dr. Lisa Larson, my major professor, is the person I must thank first and foremost for
everything from welcoming me as her student in the beginning, to providing constant guidance
and support throughout my graduate school career. She has both challenged me and respected me
in every way, for which I am very grateful and would not have had it any other way. I also thank
my committee members, Drs. Patrick Armstrong and Frederick Lorenz, for their enthusiasm and
willingness to offer conceptual and statistical guidance throughout this project (and for always
welcoming me when I’ve shown up unannounced in their offices with questions). Many thanks
go to Dr. Carolyn Cutrona as well, for her Helpance in funding this study.
I would also like to acknowledge my cohort, as my friendships with the three of them have
been crucial sources of comfort and stability in my graduate school experience over the past two
and a half years. In addition, I’m very grateful for the statistical support so willingly provided by
Yi Du, and the wonderful guidance, emotional support, and comic relief provided by my
academic big bro, Dusty M. F. Baker.
Finally and most importantly, I thank my parents, Moe and Rick, my sister, Sarah, and my
brother, Jared, for being the most influential sources of support and inspiration in my life.
Everything I do and everything I hope to become is because of them. “With you by my side, I
can have anything I need.”
vi
ABSTRACT
Several career decision-making models describe an ongoing career decision Assessment
process in which career decisions are constantly being re-assessed as individuals gain
occupational information and self-awareness. However, the relation between career decision
certainty and knowledge of one’s chosen career has not yet been adequately examined; in part
due to the difficult nature of assessing the latter construct in large samples. The present study
objectively measured occupational knowledge in 316 college students; specifically regarding the
career each student had reportedly been considering. Objective occupational knowledge of
participants’ chosen careers significantly related to interest congruence only, and neither variable
significantly contributed to variance in career certainty. Results indicated career decision-making
self-efficacy partially drives participation in career exploration activities and perceived
occupational knowledge, and all three variables in turn contributed to the prediction of career
certainty through direct and/or indirect effects. This study was the first to objectively assess
college students’ knowledge of the careers they were actively pursuing, and the first to examine
that construct along with other important career decision-making variables. Implications and
future research directions are discussed.
1
CHAPTER ONE
INTRODUCTION
The career choice process in college students is unique since these individuals generally
have the freedom to pursue whichever careers they desire. The ideal outcome for these students
is to have a career they find fulfilling, and that ultimately provides them with high levels of
vocational satisfaction. Vocational satisfaction is consistently moderately related to life
satisfaction (Bowling, Eschleman, & Wang, 2010); thus, the importance of making a “good”
career decision is great. While the career decision process is complex, as is its relation to
satisfaction, it is an important developmental challenge and one that will likely affect a good
portion of students’ future lives.
A major construct involved in the career decision-making process is occupational
knowledge. Occupational knowledge has been defined as the accuracy one has in understanding
information related to careers (Schmitt-Wilson & Welsh, 2012); when one has this accurate
understanding, he or she is considered to be informed. Being informed about occupations has
been emphasized as an important factor in the career decision-making process (Gati, 1986;
Grotevant & Durrett, 1980; Hirschi & Lage, 2007; Super, 1983). In order for one to make a
decision about which career might be fulfilling, one should have knowledge regarding the
careers available to him or her, and also regarding the specific activities and responsibilities
involved in those careers. When one has sufficient accurate occupational information, he or she
is able to make an informed decision regarding which career to pursue; he or she is
knowledgeable of the requirements needed to obtain his or her career-of-interest, the daily
activities and responsibilities of the job, and the types of environments one can work in. With
2
this knowledge, students can evaluate the degree to which careers-of-interest will fit with their
personal preferences.
Several career decision-making models explain an ongoing career decision Assessment
process in which career decisions are constantly being re-assessed as individuals gain
occupational information and self-awareness (Hirschi & Lage, 2007). Following these models,
one would expect increasing occupational knowledge to lead individuals to either persist along
the same career path or choose an alternative path. In turn, greater persistence along the same
career path is reflected in one’s degree of career certainty. Career certainty has been described as
the commitment individuals have to their decisions to pursue specific careers (Daniels, Clifton,
Perry, Mandzuk, & Hall, 2006). It is generally considered to be one of the final stages in the
career decision-making process, followed only by the planning and implementation stages
(Hirschi & Lage, 2007).
Despite the centrality of career certainty in the career decision-making process, its link to
occupational knowledge has not yet been directly examined in terms of individuals’ knowledge
about their chosen careers. This oversight seems to be due in part to a lack of adequate methods
to assess occupational knowledge. Three general types of assessment exist in the literature: those
that gauge self-estimated levels of knowledge, those that objectively test either general career
information or general aspects of common jobs, and those that objectively test specific jobs’
activities and responsibilities. All have inherent problems; namely, none are able to adequately
and efficiently measure the accuracy of job-specific knowledge in terms of activities and
responsibilities in large participant samples. Further, not only has the literature overlooked the
relation between occupational knowledge and career certainty, it has failed to address the
3
overarching question of whether or not college students are sufficiently knowledgeable of the
careers they are choosing to pursue.
The Present Study
The purpose of the present study was three-fold. First, I sought to address a hole in the
occupational knowledge literature regarding its objective measurement and its relation to career
certainty in college students. Second, since it was the first study to objectively measure
occupational knowledge of college students’ chosen careers, I sought to explore the relations
between that knowledge and four additional vocational constructs: 1) Interest congruence,
defined as the degree to which a person’s interests match those of a given work environment
(Spokane & Cruza-Guet, 2005); 2) career decision-making self-efficacy, defined as one’s belief
that he or she is able to successfully engage in and complete tasks necessary to career decisionmaking (Betz, 2000); 3) career exploration activities participants reported, a behavioral indicator
of participants’ engagement in career exploration; and 4) perceived occupational knowledge, a
subjective self-assessment of occupational knowledge which has been frequently used in studies
as a measure of “occupational knowledge.” Third, the present study sought to examine these four
variables alongside occupational knowledge in terms of their roles in predicting career certainty.
While vocational researchers have made great strides in understanding the choices people
make in their career pursuits, little attention has been given to examining whether college
students are knowledgeable about their future occupations. Some students may be confident that
their interests are leading them in the right direction, and others may be choosing careers based
on insufficient or inaccurate information. These individuals may be at risk of being dissatisfied
with their occupations once they enter the workforce and realize the inaccuracy of their
expectations. Overall, the present study sought to gain a better understanding of the factors
4
involved in making career decisions in college students. A path model predicting career certainty
was used to test specific mediation hypotheses involving occupational knowledge and the four
other variables-of-interest.
Hypotheses
Four mediation hypotheses involving the two primary variables-of-interest, objective
occupational knowledge and career certainty, as well as the additional four secondary variablesof-interest were tested using multiple regression.
Career decision-making self-efficacy has been actively researched over the past thirty
years in relation to the process of career decision-making, but has not yet been examined
alongside occupational knowledge as defined be the present study. Previous research found
lower task performance self-efficacy to be associated with low commitment to goals (Locke,
Frederick, Lee, & Bobko, 1984). The present study sought to build on this finding and examine
the relation between career decision-making self-efficacy and career certainty in the context of
occupational knowledge. Based on Locke and colleagues’ findings, lower career decisionmaking self-efficacy was expected to be associated with lower career certainty, as certainty can
be thought of as a proxy for goal commitment. Bandura and Cervone (1986) have also found that
some individuals with high performance self-efficacy tended to feel content with performance on
experimental tasks despite receiving feedback that they only nearly succeeded. These individuals
lacked the motivation required to achieve success in their task performance, with their high selfefficacy causing them to settle for sub-par performance. This phenomenon was also examined by
Salomon (1984) who found that high performance self-efficacy individuals increased their
performance when they considered the task to be difficult, but chose to decrease their
performance effort when they considered the task to be easy. Translating these findings to the
5
present study, it is possible that high career decision-making self-efficacy could compel
individuals to put less effort toward obtaining accurate occupational knowledge during their
career decision processes. Some high career decision-making self-efficacy students could still
put forth the necessary effort; though, there may be others who feel a sense of contentment with
the inadequate exploration steps they have taken thus far. These “at risk” students may view
career decision-making process tasks as easy and not necessary outlets for their effort, resulting
in occupational knowledge deficits. In these ways, career decision-making self-efficacy may
contribute to both objective occupational knowledge and career certainty, while objective
occupational knowledge also contributes to career certainty.
H1. Objective occupational knowledge would partially mediate the relation between
career decision-making self-efficacy and career certainty.
Consistent with findings presented here regarding task performance self-efficacy, the
present study predicted career decision-making self-efficacy would be involved in both the
seeking out occupational knowledge, as well as the process of determining one’s level of
certainty about a particular career path. Thus, career decision-making self-efficacy was predicted
to significantly relate to both objective occupational knowledge and career certainty, with
objective occupational knowledge partially mediating its relation with career certainty. Career
decision-making theories (Hirschi & Lage, 2007), however, suggest more variables are involved
in the complex prediction of career certainty in college students. The second hypothesis
predicted career exploration activities would play a role in the relation between career decisionmaking self-efficacy and objective occupational knowledge.
H2. Career exploration activities would partially mediate the relation between career
decision-making self-efficacy and objective occupational knowledge.
6
This hypothesis proposed a possible reason for the partial mediation predicted in
Hypothesis 1, which posited that career certainty is a function of career decision-making selfefficacy, objective occupational knowledge, and an indirect effect of career decision-making
self-efficacy through objective occupational knowledge. The number of reported career
exploration activities were posited as a mechanism for which career decision-making selfefficacy drives one to seek out objective occupational knowledge.
The third hypothesized mediation effect was of interest congruence on the relation
between objective occupational knowledge and career certainty. Congruence theory posits that
individuals naturally seek out work environments they believe will be compatible with their own
interests, leading them toward occupational satisfaction (Holland, 1997). Thus, when individuals
are able to find occupations with characteristics compatible with their own interests, they are
more likely to be satisfied with their choices. The literature has provided moderate support for
this theory (Tranberg, Slane, & Ekeberg, 1993); though, it is important to note that assessing
one’s congruence with his or her chosen occupation is an internal judgment. Individuals must
assess their own levels of congruence as they navigate through the career decision process.
Conceptually, one would expect increasing occupational knowledge to Help individuals in
assessing which career will yield the highest congruence. Therefore, if greater knowledge leads
to greater likelihood of achieving interest congruence, one might expect level of career certainty
would positively relate to both objective occupational knowledge and interest congruence,
leading to the third hypothesis.
H3. Interest congruence would partially mediate the relation between objective
occupational knowledge and career certainty.
Similar to the career decision-making self-efficacy partial mediation hypothesis,
Hypothesis 3 predicted objective occupational knowledge would relate to career certainty both
7
directly and indirectly through interest congruence. Additionally, interest congruence was
predicted to serve as its own motivating factor, leading one to continue pursing a career path or
opt for an alternative path. In this way, interest congruence would relate directly to both
objective occupational knowledge and career certainty, adding to the model’s predictive power.
Occupational knowledge literature often utilized perceived occupational knowledge as a
proxy for objective occupational knowledge. The present study included this additional variable
in the model, hypothesizing it would be predicted by both objective occupational knowledge and
career decision-making self-efficacy through the same partial mediation mechanism presented in
Hypothesis 1 for career certainty.
H4. Objective occupational knowledge would partially mediate the relation between
career decision-making self-efficacy and perceived occupational knowledge;
perceived occupational knowledge would add significantly to the prediction of
career certainty.
Additional Analysis
After determining whether specific mediation models were supported by the data, a path
analysis was run in order to examine relations between all six variables simultaneously. The path
model (Figure 1) was recursive and sequenced based on theory. In other words, each variable
was thought to have a potential effect on later variables, but not any causal reciprocal effects
(Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003). The model contained one exogenous variable, career
decision-making self-efficacy, because it has been argued that efficacy beliefs influence and
predict behavior (e. g., Bandura, 1987; Betz, 2000). The first endogenous variable was career
exploration activities, as it was hypothesized to partially result from career decision-making selfefficacy beliefs and contribute to occupational knowledge. The second endogenous variable was
objective occupational knowledge, followed by interest congruence, perceived occupational
knowledge, and career certainty.
8
Figure 1. The hypothesized recursive path model predicting career certainty.
Implications
Significant findings would have important implications for career counselors and
university career centers. Findings in support of the hypotheses would direct career counselors
and universities to areas of need among their clients and students. Counselors would be
compelled pay special attention to career certainty levels of their clients, determining whether
they are in line with their levels of knowledge about those careers. Specifically, counselors
would want to ensure their clients are making decisions based on accurate information regarding
work activities involved in their chosen careers. Further, universities may need to bolster their
career outreaches and development courses for students in order to increase career decisionmaking self-efficacy, encourage active exploration as a way of gaining occupational knowledge,
and Helping students in finding occupations congruent with their interests.
Career decisionmaking self-efficacy
Perceived
occupational
knowledge
Interest
congruence
Career certainty
Objective
occupational
knowledge
Career
exploration
activities
9
CHAPTER TWO
LITERATURE REVIEW
The career decision-making process of college students is saturated with many different
exploration and information-gathering tasks. Central to the present discussion is the process of
obtaining occupational information about one’s chosen career, which is emphasized in many
career decision-making models. Therefore, before discussing occupational knowledge in detail,
the prominent career decision-making models are presented for a better understanding of the
broad career decision-making process.
Career Decision-Making Models
The process of career decision-making in college students has been approached from
multiple perspectives, resulting in several different models. While these models are often quite
complex, simplified versions are discussed here in order to provide a brief overview of the
theoretical foundations of the career decision-making process.
Crites (1976) identified five aspects involved in effective career decision-making. First is
accurate self-appraisal involving individuals’ self-assessment of interests, needs, and values.
Second is gathering occupational information which is necessary in order for individuals to
determine the characteristics of occupations that might fit their personal characteristics.
Gathering information about one’s self and occupations is followed by goal selection in which
individuals make decisions about which occupation(s) they prefer. The final two elements are
making future plans and problem solving. After individuals select their career goals, they must
plan how they will obtain those goals and be prepared to face problems that arise in their career
pursuits. These five elements comprise “career maturity,” which Crites introduced as an
overarching developmental characteristic that allows individuals to effectively engage in the
10
career decision-making process. Crites argued that by engaging in these five elements, and
thereby demonstrating career maturity, individuals will be able to make effective career
decisions.
Harren (1979) identified the need for career decision-making models specific to certain
life stages so that career developmental concepts could be integrated. Thus, his model was
created with traditional college students in mind. Central to his model, Harren outlined a
sequential four-stage process. First is Awareness, during which one engages in appraisal of one’s
past, present, and future, and includes analyses of one’s current course of action, decisionmaking confidence, and environmental pressures. Next is Planning, characterized by information
gathering about one’s career decision and one’s self. Once enough information has been
acquired, the individual begins narrowing alternatives down to a specific choice in preparation
for the next stage. This stage is followed by Commitment, in which one integrates the choice into
his or her self-concept and begins planning to implement the choice. Finally, Implementation
occurs when the individual carries out the choice and evaluates it, returning to the beginning of
the process if he or she is not satisfied.
Alternatively, Gati (1986) has presented a model characterized by systematic elimination
in order to identify small subsets of career alternatives, effectively simplifying the career
decision-making process. In Gati’s Sequential Elimination Model, occupations are considered to
be made up of various aspects which determine whether or not a given occupation will be
included in or excluded from the career decision-making process. For example, if an individual
considers prestige important, occupations may be excluded if they are considered low in prestige.
Thus, the first stage of Gati’s model involves the identification of occupational aspects one
considers relevant to his or her career decision. Aspects are then ranked by importance and
11
occupation alternatives are eliminated according to the guidelines set by aspect rankings. This
process continues until the list of potential careers is of acceptable length. A strength of Gati’s
model is that it inherently forces individuals to engage in career exploration, providing them with
information about the aspects of occupations they find important.
An additional model, the Cognitive Information-Processing Approach for career
decision-making (Sampson, Reardon, Peterson, & Lenz, 2004), presents a series of phases that
individuals cycle through until a career decision has been made. Individuals first identify the
problem (need to choose a career; Communication), identify all components of the problem
(Analysis), formulate possible solutions for the problem (career alternatives; Synthesis), rank
alternative solutions (Valuing), and choose alternative to implement (Execution). Individuals
then return to the Communication stage to evaluate their decisions, continuing on with the
process again if problems are identified.
More recently, Hirschi and Lage (2007) noted the similarities existing among the
different career decision-making models. They sought to combine the themes identified in
previous models, boiling them down to six basic phases. In Phase 1, individuals become aware of
the need to make a career decision. Hirschi and Lage note the importance of this stage, since
some individuals do not become concerned with career decision-making in the first place. During
Phase 2, career alternatives are generated based on individuals’ interests, skills, and values. In
Phase 3, Alternatives are then eliminated in order to reduce possibilities to a reasonable number
in preparation for the exploration phase. Phase 4 constitutes exploration and further reduction of
career alternatives. In Phase 5, individuals make a choice and begin committing to it. Finally, in
Phase 6 individuals are committed to their choices and implementing action plans. This phase
was intentionally included in order to distinguish between individuals still engaged in the career
12
decision-making process (who would cycle back through the phases) from those who have firmly
committed to careers.
As Hirschi and Lage (2007) identified, the different career decision-making models have
substantial overlap. Of most interest to the present discussion are the middle phases, Phases 2-5,
in which individuals are engaged in the actual career decision-making process rather than in the
pre- and post-decision-making phases. During these phases, individuals are gathering
information about themselves and potential occupations, identifying which occupations might be
appropriate, and then narrowing down those occupations until ultimately one remains.
Importantly, the role of information is salient throughout these phases. Not only do individuals
need to gather sufficient, accurate information about all the occupations available to them, they
must continually process that information in tandem with the information they have about
themselves. The information processing continues until a career choice is made, at which point
individuals will likely have to gather more information for planning and implementation
purposes. This process, common among the various career decision-making models, makes
salient the important role of information in one’s ability to make a career decision.
The Role of Information in Career Decision-Making
The career decision-making models presented here have been developed with the purpose
of outlining how one goes about reaching a career decision when faced with alternatives (Gati,
1986). The importance of making a good career decision is great, as “there are few other
decisions that exert as profound an influence on people’s lives as the choice of a field of work or
career” (Hackett & Betz, 1995; p. 249). Gati (1986) identified four sources of problems that can
prevent one from reaching an optimal career decision: a) lack of information about one’s self or
of career alternatives, b) lack of resources to acquire the necessary information, c) cognitive
13
limitations in the decision-maker preventing information processing, and d) lack of ability in the
decision-maker to integrate and process information about one’s self and one’s career
alternatives. Central to these problem sources is information, both about the person’s interests
and the career alternatives. By gaining information about one’s self and one’s occupation of
interest, one can be confident that he or she has the information needed to make a good career
decision. While the role of self-information is beyond the scope of the present discussion, the
role of occupational information is central: When one has sufficient accurate information about
his or her occupation alternatives, he or she is able to make an informed decision regarding
which of them to pursue.
Occupational Knowledge
In light of the emphasis placed on occupational information by the career decisionmaking models, the question arises of whether or not college students are approaching career
decisions in the most efficient way. That is, whether or not they are fully engaging in the stages
laid out by the various models and acquiring the information necessary to reach an optimal
career decision. Without any constraints, efficiency may not matter. However, reality compels
one to consider the potential disadvantages to approaching career decision in an inefficient way –
i.e., making uninformed decisions to pursue occupations that may lead one to desire a change in
career paths. The purpose of measuring college students’ levels of occupational knowledge then,
is to determine whether there is a subset of students making uninformed decisions that may result
in future dissatisfaction or distress; to alert professionals of a possible “at risk” group of students.
To begin addressing the question of whether or not college students are making informed
decisions while they are engaged in the career decision-making process, a discussion of the
literature around occupational knowledge is necessary.
14
Assessment of Occupational Knowledge
Before a review of the occupational knowledge literature findings, it is important to have
an understanding of the ways in which this knowledge is assessed. The overview that follows
describes the measurement approaches that have been found in the literature. This discussion is
necessary since the generalizability of each study’s findings are limited by the assessment
method used.
The Career Maturity Inventory (CMI). The CMI (Crites, 1973, 1978) has been used to
test individuals on their knowledge of aspects of common occupations. For example, participants
would be asked if they agree or disagree with the statement, “Anita wants to be a lawyer. She
must first pass the bar exams and then go to law school.” This scale was found to be the most
commonly used measure of occupational knowledge, with seven studies incorporating it
(Achebe, 1982; Holland & Gottfredson, 1975; Johnson, Johnson, Johnson, & Yates, 1982;
Johnson, Johnson, & Yates, 1981; Jordan & Pope, 2001; Omvig & Tulloch, 1975; Pumfrey &
Schofield, 1982).
Overall, the CMI is quite useful in assessing broad occupational knowledge for the
purposes of measuring career maturity levels as defined by Crites’ (1976) model of career
decision-making. However, it does not satisfy the requirements of the present discussion in that
specific information of individuals’ chosen careers is not addressed. It seems that, while career
maturity is useful for broadly investigating individuals’ potential for making effective career
decisions, the construct is not operationally defined in way that provides a solution to the
measurement issues at hand. Specifically, the extent to which people are informed about the
careers they are choosing to pursue.
15
The Career Development Inventory (CDI). Developed by Super, Thompson,
Lindeman, Jordaan, and Myers (1981), the CDI is considered a well-established vocational
maturity assessment and has been used in eight studies for the purpose of job knowledge
measurement. Of those, only two included the subscale of interest (Hirschi, 2011; Healy &
Mourton, 1984); namely, the Planning Scale. The CDI – Planning Scale asks individuals to
identify the career they aspire to pursue and are presented with an eight-item questionnaire in
which they rated their knowledge of the job’s required abilities and education, work context,
expected pay, etc., on a five-point scale from (1) poorly to (5) very well. While this scale of the
instrument does address specific aspects of individuals’ chosen careers, the issue of job
knowledge accuracy remains. It is important to note that all the statistical conclusions made
using this scale are based on self-estimated levels of knowledge rather than on objective
assessment.
The CDI – Knowledge of Preferred Occupation scale asks individuals to choose one of a
number of occupational groups in which their career choice best fits. Individuals are then
presented with 40 multiple choice items inquiring about facets of the job related to required
abilities, interests and values fulfilled, training requirements, and the environments in which
people work. Notably, work activities involved in one’s chosen occupation were not assessed.
The CDI – Knowledge of Preferred Occupation scale certainly has potential. However, it
examines participants’ knowledge of occupational categories rather than titles, which are
relatively broad and thus the scale does not solve the assessment problem at hand. Further, it
does not inquire about work activities, which give important information about what a given job
is actually like on a daily basis. The bulk of the recent research using the CDI has been almost
exclusive to the Australian version of the CDI (Creed, Patton, & Hood, 2010; Creed, Patton, &
16
Prideaux, 2007; Patton & Creed, 2007; Patton, Spooner-Lane, & Creed, 2005; Creed & Patton,
2003), which, unfortunately, omitted the Knowledge of Preferred Occupational Group scale in its
development (Lokan, 1984; Sadeghi, Baghban, Bahrami, Ahmadi, & Creed, 2011). Therefore,
measurement of aspects of one’s preferred occupational group knowledge has not been
extensively investigated with the CDI.
An earlier version of the CDI which included a scale labeled “Part V” (Super et al., 1981)
was used by one study (Crowley, 1981). The scale consisted of 30 multiple choice items
designed to gauge participants’ knowledge of aspects of common occupations, such as the tools
or equipment used, training needed, duties, and employment trends.
The Jobs-Career Key (JCK). The JCK (Blank, 1978) was used in one study (Taylor,
1985) and is comprised of 147 multiple choice items across four occupational domains
(economic factors, education and training, job parts, and worker relationships) for 18
traditionally feminine and 18 traditionally masculine occupations. This measure is strong in its
ability to assess knowledge of various aspects of a variety of occupations, but it does not inquire
about specific activities or responsibilities within those jobs and is limited to 36 occupations.
The Cognitive Vocational Maturity Test (CVMT). The CVMT (Westbrook & ParryHill, 1973) is a 120 multiple choice-item measure with six subscales used in three studies
(Fouad, 1995; Westbrook, 1976; Westbrook, Elrod, & Wynne, 1996). The Duties subscale
objectively measures individuals’ knowledge of the typical duties performed in various wellknow occupations. For example, one item asks “Which one fills prescriptions for drugs and
medicine? a) Chemist, b) Physicist, c) Pharmacist, d) Pharmacologist, or e) I don’t know.”
Additional scales assess Fields of Work (e.g., “Which one of the following is not in the field of
construction?), Work Conditions (e.g., “Which one of the following does not have to work with
17
tools?”), Education Required (e.g., “A college education is not required to be a …”), Attributes
Required (e.g., “Imagination is most important in which occupation?”), and Job Selection which
presents short narratives describing a person’s skills and personality, asking participants to
indicate which occupation would be most fitting. This method of assessment is relatively strong
as it inquires about details specific to a occupations. However, it does not assess students’ levels
of knowledge of the careers they are interested in pursuing, limiting the knowledge assessment to
a small sample of common occupations. Furthermore, the studies that utilized this measure
included relatively young samples. Eighth grade (Fouad, 1995) and ninth grade (Westbrook,
1976) students were assessed in two studies, with only one study sampling eleventh and twelfth
grade students (Westbrook et al., 1996).
The Career Planning Inventory (CPI). Developed by Westbrook (1997), this 10-item
scale was designed to measure individuals’ breadth of career knowledge. It was found to be used
in only one study (Westbrook & Sloan, 2006) and is comprised of multiple choice questions in
which participants choose the occupation that fits the job task description (e.g., “Which of the
following designs buildings?” Mechanic, architect, surveyor, engineer, or “I don’t know”). While
a good objective measure of broad occupational knowledge, this scale suffers from the same
issues as the CVMT.
The Job Knowledge Survey (JKS). Developed by Loesch (1977), the JKS was found to
be used in two studies (Loesch & Sampson, 1978; Sampson & Loesch, 1981) and consists of 48
occupational titles sampled randomly from each of the RIASEC (Realistic, Investigative,
Artistic, Social, Enterprising, and Conventional; Holland, 1997) interest domains which
participants are asked to rate the occupational titles’ levels of emphasis on people, data, and
things. This measure is strong in that it asks individuals to separate occupational titles based on
18
the general domains of typical job activities. However, the measure does little to assess
knowledge of the occupations’ activities and responsibilities beyond broad categorization.
The Career Decision Profile (CDP). The CDP (Jones, 1989), has been used by two
studies to measure several career development variables, including occupational knowledge
(Duffy & Kingaman, 2009; Multon, Heppner, & Lapan, 1995). It includes three items gauging
“Knowledge about Occupations and Training” on a 5-point scale of agreement (e.g., “I do not
feel I know enough about the occupations I am considering”). While the items in this measure
successfully inquire about participants’ levels of information about their chosen careers, the
subjective nature makes it difficult to interpret responses.
The Occupational Knowledge Scale (OKS). Adapted from Tracey’s Personal Globe
Inventory (2002) for use in one study (Rohlfing, Nota, Ferrari, Soresi, & Tracey, 2012), this
measure is comprised of 18 occupational titles of which individuals are to rate their knowledge
from (1) I don’t know at all to (5) I know a lot. This method suffers from the same validity issue
as that of other self-estimate measures; any conclusions made from its use must be
contextualized so as not to confuse objective occupational knowledge with self-estimations.
Miscellaneous knowledge assessments. Two studies were found that utilized personnel
selection knowledge tests (Ones & Viswesvaran, 2007; Van Iddekinge, Putka, & Campbell,
2011) developed by subject matter experts, human resources specialists, or industrialorganizational psychologists. Using a multiple choice format, they tested specific tasks and
activities common in the occupations of interest. While this approach is an improvement upon
the methods used by other researchers since it assesses specific job activities and tasks, creating
such comprehensive job knowledge measures for any career-diverse sample would be quite time-
19
consuming. For this reason, this method has only been used with very specific populations and
lacks generalizability.
Two studies used a measurement method in which individuals record their knowledge of
six dimensions of 20 occupational titles (Schmitt-Wilson & Welsh, 2012; Walls, 2000). The six
dimensions, time (training, preparation), availability (positions, jobs), earnings, physical
(strength, stamina), mental (thinking, knowledge), and status (respect, prestige), have unique
response scale or scales (e.g., the mental dimension was measured by three subscales assessing
reasoning development, mathematical development, and language development). Responses are
standardized based on information from the Dictionary of Occupational Titles. This measure taps
into various important aspects of occupational knowledge; however, it does not specifically
address individuals’ careers of interest, nor does it assess knowledge of specific occupational
activities or responsibilities which hold important information for career decision-making.
Finally, one study measured occupational knowledge through individual interviews
(DeFleur & Menke, 1975). Ten occupations were assessed in terms of knowledge of job
activities, nature of the work, specializations within the occupation, education and training
requirements, salary, prestige, and job availability outlook. Accuracy scores were assigned based
on information from the Occupational Outlook Handbook. This methodology is good compared
to other approaches in the literature, but it is also largely impractical and inefficient for large
sample sizes. Assessment methods are needed that are able to incorporate the level of specificity
that these interviews provided, while remaining usable in research with large samples.
Summary of Assessment Methods. It seems that the current psychological measurement
of job knowledge is largely flawed. Three general types of assessment exist in the literature:
those that gauge self-estimated levels of knowledge, those that objectively test either general
20
career information or relatively common-knowledge aspects of specific jobs, and those that
objectively test specific jobs’ activities and responsibilities. All have inherent problems; namely,
none are able to adequately and efficiently measure accuracy of job-specific knowledge in terms
of activities and responsibilities in large participant samples. Those used in personnel selection
most closely resemble ideal occupational knowledge assessments, but cannot realistically be
used in studies involving diverse career choices. Individual interviews have been conducted in
past studies, but are far too inefficient and unrealistic to employ in most empirical studies.
Notably, many aspects of occupational knowledge have been assessed in some way; though none
of the available literature has investigated the extent to which college students are
knowledgeable about the specific job activities they will encounter after entering their
occupation of choice.
Review of the Occupational Knowledge Literature
A literature search using PsycInfo and the term knowledge with career, job, or
occupation and college, undergraduate, high school, or middle school yielded 190 peer-reviewed
articles written in English. Of those 190, 159 articles were excluded because they were deemed
irrelevant to the present discussion. Many studies were excluded because they did not report
results related to occupational knowledge (e.g., Greenhaus, Hawkins, & Brenner, 1983;
Patterson, Curtis, & Reid, 2008; Reardon, Senier, & Lewis, 1972; Theophilides, Terenzini, &
Lorang, 1984), were based on operational definitions of occupational knowledge that deviated
from the definition used by the present discussion (e.g., by measuring knowledge of general job
market patterns or career development tasks, having participants list occupational titles as a
measure of knowledge; e.g., Gunkel, Schlaegel, Langella, & Peluchette, 2010; Rottinghaus,
Buelow, Matyja, & Schneider, 2012; Rottinghaus, Day, & Borgen, 2005; Watson, Foxcroft, &
21
Horn, 1997), or if the target population was not assessed (e.g., middle school teachers’
knowledge was measured; e.g., Arzi & White, 2008; Beswick, Callingham, & Watson, 2012;
Gable, Tonelson, Sheth, Wilson, & Park, 2012). Empirical findings presented in the remaining
31 articles are reviewed below, organized by finding categories.
Career intervention effects. Ten studies were found that investigated the effectiveness
of career interventions, measured at least in part by changes in occupational knowledge. These
interventions have been found to be effective in increasing self-estimations of occupational
knowledge in college students on academic probation (Williams, 2011), undergraduate
psychology students (Atchley, Hooker, Kroska, & Gilmour, 2012), primarily Caucasian middle
school students (Crowley, 1981; Johnson, Johnson, & Yates, 1981; Omvig, Tulloch, & Thomas,
1975), Black economically disadvantaged middle school students (Johnson, Johnson, Johnson, &
Yates, 1982), ethnically diverse eighth grade students (Fouad, 1995), and middle school students
in England (Pumfrey & Schofield, 1982). Holland’s Self-Directed Search (1971) was used as a
career intervention in two studies, though neither found significant intervention effects (Healy &
Mourton, 1984; Holland & Gottfredson, 1975).
Age/grade and gender. Six studies analyzed the effects of age and grade on occupational
knowledge levels. Cross-sectional studies have found that occupational knowledge increases by
age or grade in samples of 7th
-10th grade Nigerian students (Achebe, 1982), 6th and 8th grade
students (Omvig et al., 1975), 3
rd, 6th, 9th, and 12th grade students (Walls, 2000), 4th
-7
th grade
rural students (Schmitt-Wilson & Welsh, 2012), and high school males (DeFleur & Menke,
1975). However, Fouad’s (1995) one-year longitudinal study using the CVMT did not produce
significant effects related to age in a sample of ethnically diverse eighth grade students. Fouad
also reported no gender differences in her sample. Conversely, Miller and Hayward (2006) found
22
that high school females’ self-estimations of occupational knowledge were higher than those of
males, and two studies reported higher levels of objectively measured occupational knowledge in
females (Rohlfing, et al., 2012; Westbrook & Sloan, 2006). Notably, none of these studies
looked at age or gender in relation to knowledge of individuals’ chosen occupations.
Cognitive ability/academic achievement. Six studies examined occupational knowledge
scores in relation to participants’ cognitive abilities or academic achievement. Jordan and Pope
(2001) found longitudinal evidence that broad, objectively-measured occupational knowledge in
their sample of 17-year-olds was significantly predicted by mothers’ vocabulary when
participants were at age 4 and participants’ vocabulary at age 14. Other studies have found
broad, objectively-measured occupational knowledge to be moderately and positively related to
achievement measured by self-reported high school achievement (Westbrook, Elrod, & Wynne,
1996), middle school grade point average (GPA; Schmitt-Wilson & Welsh, 2012), and high
school GPA (DeFleur & Menke, 1975; Westbrook & Sloan, 2006). Additionally, Van Iddekinge
et al. (2011) reported a moderate correlation between cognitive aptitude measured by the Armed
Forces Qualification Test and Army job activity-specific occupational knowledge (r = .52).
Performance. Three studies investigated the relation between occupational knowledge
and performance. Ones and Viswesvaran (2007) found that the job activity-specific knowledge
tests created for factory worker selection related significantly to “maximal performance” (r =
.42), operationally defined by participants’ observed performance during a hands-on work
sample test. Similarly, Van Iddekinge et al. (2011) found a significant relation between Army job
activity-specific occupational knowledge and task proficiency (r = .29), measured by multiple
ratings of task performance. One study sought to determine the unique contributions of
occupational knowledge and cognitive ability to task performance (Palumbo, Miller, Shalin, &
23
Steele-Johnson, 2005). College students’ scores on a cognitive ability test and a truck driving job
knowledge test both separately predicted performance on a truck dispatching job simulation task,
explaining 12% and 26% of performance variance, respectively. Additionally, cognitive ability
accounted for 23% of the variance in job knowledge. However, after controlling for the effect of
knowledge on performance, cognitive ability no longer made a significant contribution in
predicting performance. The authors concluded that job knowledge fully mediated the effect of
cognitive ability on task performance in this context, calling into question the findings of Ones
and Viswesvaran, and Van Iddekinge and colleagues.
Exposure/experience. Two studies addressed the relation between exposure to, or
experience with, occupations on occupational knowledge. Pumfrey and Schofield (1982)
administered the CMI after middle school students participated in week-long real-world work
experiences and compared their scores to students in a control group who did not have the work
experience. The authors found a significant main effect of the intervention on broad, objectivelymeasured occupational knowledge, suggesting that even one week of real-world work experience
can increase levels of general occupational knowledge. Additionally, DeFleur and Menke (1975)
found that out of ten common traditionally-male occupations, high school males were most
accurate in their occupational knowledge of truck driving (57.51% accuracy) and least accurate
for accounting (34.46% accuracy). The authors suggest that knowledge accuracy in this context
may be related to the degree of exposure individuals have to a given occupation, supported by
the finding that participants had greater accurate knowledge related to an occupation they likely
see frequently compared to one that they do not. Importantly, DeFleur and Menke utilized
individual participant interviews which included open-ended questions regarding the activities
involved in each of the ten occupations assessed.
24
Vocational interests. Five studies have examined the relation between vocational
interests and occupational knowledge to some degree. Rohlfing et al. (2012) assessed 3
rd
-6
th
grade students’ general self-estimated occupational knowledge and domain-specific knowledge
estimations separated by Personal Globe Inventory domains (People/Things, Ideas/Data, and
Prestige; Tracey, 2002). Significant correlations were found between domain knowledge scores
and corresponding interest ratings: People/Things knowledge was positively and significantly
related to People/Things interest (r = .30); though the opposite pattern was found for the
Ideas/Data domain (r = -.12). These results indicate that participants tend to rate their knowledge
higher in domains they are interested in. Contrary to these findings however, Loesch and
Sampson (1978) found no relationship between RIASEC (Realistic, Investigative, Artistic,
Social, Enterprising, and Conventional; Holland, 1997) interest scores and corresponding domain
knowledge scores. These results conflict with those of Rohlfing et al., suggesting that knowledge
may not be driven by interest. Notably, Loesch and Sampson produced these findings using an
objective knowledge measure while Rohlfing and colleagues employed self-estimates.
A different approach to the interest-knowledge relationship was taken by Westbrook and
Parry-Hill (1973). They used the six CVMT subscales (Duties, Fields of Work, Work
Conditions, Education Required, Attributes Required, and Job Selection) that objectively and
broadly assess knowledge of different aspects of common occupations. The authors found
significant differences on all subscales between middle school participants who had chosen
“appropriate” career choices and those who had not. Career decisions were considered
appropriate when they were of the same interest domain as the individual’s reported vocational
interests (categorized based on the Kuder Preference Record; Kuder, 1944). This finding
suggests that more knowledge of common occupations is related to one’s ability to identify
25
career paths that are congruent with his or her vocational interests. Conversely, Holland and
Gottfredson (1975) found no relations between objectively-measured broad occupational
knowledge and degree of congruence between one’s interests and career choice (categorized
based on the RIASEC domains). Thus this relation remains equivocal.
Van Iddekinge and colleagues (2011) examined vocational interests and occupational
knowledge specific to common activities and responsibilities of Army soldiers. Entered as a
composite in a regression predicting occupational knowledge, RIASEC interest scores measured
by the Work Preferences Inventory (Amabile, Hill, Hennessey, & Tighe, 1994) yielded an R
2
of
.46, indicating that 46% of the variance in occupational knowledge was explained by vocational
interest. Individually, Realistic (prefer hands-on, practical activities) and Investigative (prefer
scientific, intellectual activities) interests significantly related to occupational knowledge (r = .20
and .29, respectively). No other RIASEC types yielded significant relations. These findings
supported the authors’ hypothesis in that Army-related interests significantly related to Army
occupational knowledge, suggesting that greater interest in a given field may lead one to seek out
and acquire more knowledge of activities in that field. This implication is especially important
given the associations between occupational knowledge and performance. It may be that interests
serve to drive knowledge acquisition, which in turn drives job performance; a model that has
received great support in the literature (e.g., Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997; Holland, 1997). Of
further importance is the similarity between this finding and that of Westbrook and Parry-Hill:
occupational knowledge is greater when one’s interests and career choice are congruent. In
conjunction with previously discussed relations between occupational knowledge and
performance, these studies support the notion that occupational knowledge is an important
variable within the career decision-making process.
26
Career certainty. This career decision-making construct is arguably one of the more
important, as it is generally the goal of the career decision-making process. The final stage in all
of the career decision-making models described previously involves commitment to a career
decision and subsequent planning and implementation. Career certainty, or career decidedness in
some of the literature, is the degree of certainty one has regarding his or her chosen career (Durr
& Tracey, 2009). This variable has been assessed alongside occupational knowledge in four
studies. One study reported a small but significant correlation between career certainty and selfestimated levels of occupational knowledge (r = .24; Multon et al., 1995), but another study
examining objectively-measured occupational knowledge and certainty yielded no relation (r =
.00; Westbrook & Sloan, 2006). Similarly, a study of high school students by Westbrook and
colleagues (1996) failed to find a significant relation between the CVMT – Duties scale and
career certainty (r = .20, N = 91). However, the same authors found a strong correlation between
certainty (measured by the Career Choice Questionnaire – Stability, Certainty, and Satisfaction
of One’s Career Choice; Westbrook, 1985) and self-estimated knowledge of the attributes (e.g.,
abilities and education) required for participants’ chosen careers (r = .65). This self-estimated
knowledge of attributes scale was comprised of seven Likert scale items from (1) definitely no to
(5) definitely yes in which participants responded to statements such as, Know the duties required
in career choice and Have information about career choice. Conceptually, this relatively strong
relation makes sense; greater knowledge of one’s career choice is associated with greater career
certainty. However, given that self-estimated knowledge was used, it remains unknown whether
participants’ self-estimations are accurate.
One study assessed the effects of a teacher observation intervention on undergraduate
secondary education students (Thomas & Bruning, 1984). Thirty-three of the 224 participants
27
dropped out of the program after the intervention, and had significantly lower career certainty
than their peers who remained in the program. The dropout students were not statistically
different regarding career certainty before the intervention; though, after observing the
occupation they were pursuing at the time, their certainty levels decreased significantly. This
finding may suggest that some of the students did not have a good understanding of what
teaching career would be like and, once they obtained that information, they no longer wanted to
pursue it.
Other career development constructs. Various additional career-related variables have
been examined in relation to objectively-measured broad occupational knowledge. Sampson and
Loesch (1981) assessed occupational knowledge’s relation to work values, but found no patterns
among RIASEC domains of knowledge and domains of work values. Achebe (1982) found that
urban middle and high school students in Nigeria had more occupational knowledge than their
rural counterparts, possibly relating to the number or type of occupations students were exposed
to in their communities. Hirschi (2011) found that self-estimated knowledge of seventh grade
students’ chosen careers significantly correlated with generalized self-efficacy and self-esteem,
although the magnitude of the correlations were small (r = .27 and .26, respectively). These
relations make sense given the subjective self-report nature of the occupational knowledge
measure: Higher self-efficacy and self-esteem may lead participants to rate their levels of
occupational knowledge has higher than those with lower self-efficacy and self-esteem.
Regarding competence perceptions, Rohlfing and colleagues (2012) found significant
correlations between the People/Things knowledge domain and People/Things competence
perceptions (r = .25) of 3rd
-6
th grade students. However, these results were based on self-
28
estimates of knowledge for individual occupational titles, indicating that these students tend to
perceive higher competence in the occupations with which they are familiar.
Knowledge of chosen occupations. Of particular interest to the present discussion is the
extent to which individuals are making informed career decisions; that is, whether or not they are
knowledgeable of the occupations they are choosing to pursue. Much of the occupational
literature concerns general knowledge of common occupations, and only three studies have
examined individuals’ knowledge of their chosen occupations. A significant relation was found
between female participants’ self-estimates of knowledge and their preferences for occupations,
while no significant relation was found for male participants (Miller & Hayward, 2006). This
result suggests that females estimate their levels of knowledge in accordance with their levels of
preference for those occupations; though, it is unknown whether their self-estimates of
knowledge are accurate or if they perceive more knowledge about occupations which they prefer.
Westbrook and colleagues (1996) found a significant relation between self-estimated knowledge
of the attributes required in participants’ chosen occupations and occupational knowledge
measured by the CVMT – Duties scale (r = .28). The CVMT – Duties scale objectively measures
general knowledge of duties required in a subset of common occupations. Thus, this result
indicates that individuals with higher general knowledge of common occupational duties selfestimate higher knowledge levels of the attributes (e.g., abilities and education) required for their
chosen careers.
One study was found that objectively measured college students’ knowledge of their
chosen careers to an extent. Healy and Mourton (1984) employed the CDI – Knowledge of
Preferred Occupation scale, which asks participants to choose one of a number of occupational
groups in which their career choice best fits. Participants are then presented with 40 multiple
29
choice items inquiring about facets of the job related to required abilities, interests and values
fulfilled, training requirements, and work environment. Notably, work activities were not
assessed. The Self-Directed Search (SDS; Holland, 1971) was used as an intervention and
hypothesized to increase career exploration, with results indicating no effect of the intervention
on occupational knowledge. Descriptive statistics for the experimental (M = 2.06, SD = .93) and
control (M = 1.87, SD = .89) groups were reported; however, interpretation of these statistics is
uncertain since, traditionally, the CDI standardizes scores to a mean of 100 (Hansen, 1985).
Self-estimates and objective measures of knowledge. As Kruger and Dunning (1999)
established, oftentimes individuals who are unaware of certain information are also unaware of
their knowledge deficit. After testing this effect using an English grammar test, Kruger and
Dunning found that bottom-quartile participants scored, on average, in the 10th percentile, while
they had estimated their test score to be in the 67th percentile. Translating this concept to the
present discussion of occupational knowledge, three studies were found that investigated both
objective and self-estimated levels of occupational knowledge.
Yanico and Mihlbauer (1983) examined the relationship between self-estimated and
objectively-measured occupational knowledge with regards to gender traditional and
nontraditional occupations. After completing the Jobs-Career Key test of 36 occupations, college
student participants were asked to indicate how much they thought they knew about each
occupation on a scale from 1 to 100. No significant differences were found between estimated
and objective knowledge levels; however, all participants underestimated knowledge levels of
their respective nontraditional occupations. These results were later replicated (Yanico & Hardin,
1986) and suggest that college students are accurate in their estimations of knowledge regarding
the traditional and nontraditional occupational domains on the Jobs-Career Key. Importantly, this
30
study did not assess knowledge of chosen occupations. Participants’ objective and self-estimated
knowledge was only measured in relation to common gender-stereotyped occupations.
Grotevant and Durrett (1980) reported findings from their investigation of high school
seniors. Participants reported their first-choice career, stated how well they understood that
career on a 1 to 5 Likert scale, then indicated their educational goals. Students’ vocational
interests were also assessed for the purpose of measuring their occupational congruence.
Grovetant and Durrett found that, of the students who claimed to have at least a “considerable”
understanding of their first career choice, only half of them planned to obtain the occupation’s
required level of education.
While the literature on the accuracy of self-estimated occupational knowledge is not
abundant, Grovetant and Durrett’s study provides solid evidence of a discrepancy solely between
self-estimated knowledge and appropriate educational goals. If high school seniors do not have
enough knowledge about a career to plan to obtain the required education, one might expect an
even larger discrepancy between their perceived and actual knowledge of the daily job activities
their occupational choice entails. This expectation is concerning and has not been addressed thus
far in the literature, largely due to the paucity of appropriate assessment tools currently in use.
Summary
There are important findings from this literature that can be used to inform future
research on the topic of occupational knowledge. The self-estimated knowledge literature has
shown that subjective occupational knowledge is slightly related to self-efficacy and self-esteem
(e.g., Hirschi, 2011) and strongly related to career certainty (e.g., Westbrook et al., 1996).
However, the studies utilizing objectively measured occupational knowledge provide somewhat
more useful information going forward. Objectively measured occupational knowledge has been
31
shown to be related to academic achievement (e.g., Schmitt-Wilson & Welsh, 2012; Westbrook,
Elrod, & Wynne, 1996), work experience (e.g., Pumfrey & Schofield, 1982), job performance
(e.g., Ones & Viswesvaran, 2007), and interests (e.g., Rohlfing et al., 2012; Van Iddekinge et al.,
2011). It has been shown to increase by age and grade (e.g., Achebe, 1982; Pumfrey &
Schofield, 1982) and to predict career decisions congruent with one’s interests (e.g., Westbrook
& Parry-Hill, 1973). Further, there is evidence that occupational knowledge is related to work
exposure (e.g., DeFleur & Menke, 1975), cognitive aptitude, and job proficiency (Van Iddekinge
et al., 2011).
These studies have objectively investigated many aspects of occupational knowledge,
including training and education requirements, job availability, earnings and prestige, mental and
physical requirements, and interests and values fulfilled. Job activities have even been assessed
on a job-specific level by one study (DeFleur & Menke, 1975) but the authors did not report the
level of specificity used in coding participant responses nor did they assess participants’
knowledge of their chosen occupations. Furthermore, this study utilized interview methods,
making it quite impractical for most large sample research endeavors. Overall, while many
aspects of occupational knowledge have been assessed in some way, none of the available
literature has investigated the extent to which college students are knowledgeable about the
careers they are choosing to pursue.
Measurement Solutions
It seems that the current psychological measurement of job knowledge is largely flawed.
Three general types of assessment exist in the literature: those that gauge self-estimated levels of
knowledge, those that objectively test either general career information or relatively commonknowledge aspects of specific jobs, and those that objectively test specific jobs’ activities and
32
responsibilities. All have inherent problems; namely, none are able to adequately and efficiently
measure accuracy of job-specific knowledge in terms of activities and responsibilities across
large participant samples. Activities and responsibilities one will encounter upon entering his or
her chosen occupation are of interest because they are often the aspects of jobs for which people
must seek information to be fully knowledgeable. Of course, the major activities of jobs tend to
be obvious; accountants work with numbers, physicians treat medical patients, interior designers
decorate rooms, etc. However, beyond these general activities are duties and responsibilities that
may not enter the minds of some individuals as they pursue these careers. Currently, there are no
practical methods of measuring these important aspects of occupational knowledge in large
samples. Those used in personnel selection most closely resemble ideal occupational knowledge
assessments, but cannot realistically be used in studies involving diverse career choices.
Individual interviews have been conducted in past studies, but are far too inefficient and
unrealistic to employ in most empirical studies.
One assessment method that could potentially address these measurement issues and
produce information regarding college students’ knowledge of the work activities involved in
their chosen occupations is inherent in the O*NET online career exploration system. The system
includes a Detailed Work Activities database which consists of a set of 41 work activities rated
by importance for each of the 974 O*NET occupational titles (National Center for O*NET
Development, 2003). Average importance ratings for each activity are scored from 0 to 100 for
each occupation based on data collected from occupation experts and random samples of
workers (Research Triangle Institute, 2006). For each item, respondents were to respond to the
question, “How important is the activity to your current job?” Respondents’ answers were then
averaged for each item to create the importance score for each work activity within a given
33
occupation. The 41 work activities are the same across 974 occupations in the database which
allows for practical, efficient comparison of occupational knowledge in large research samples.
As an example, the occupation activity importance profile for an accountant is presented in
Appendix C.
A strength of this measurement method is that it allows for the use of a profile correlation
(Q correlation; Cronbach & Gleser, 1953) in analyzing the results. The Q correlation, which
provides scores ranging from -1.00 to +1.00, would provide a standardized measurement of the
agreement between a participant’s work activity rating profile for his or her chosen occupation
and the corresponding O*NET rating profile for the occupation. A score of .00 would indicate no
pattern of similarity between profiles, scores nearing +1.00 would indicate greater profile
matches, and scores reaching -1.00 would indicate nearly opposite profiles (Armstrong, Fouad,
Rounds, & Hubert, 2010). This statistic is not descriptive of elevation differences between two
profiles, but of differences in shape. This point is important because, given that a scale from 0 to
100 is not descriptive in and of itself, participants’ knowledge would not be assessed based on
their ratings of the importance of a given activity, but rather on the importance they assign a
given activity relative to all the other activities.
Career Certainty
Selected Literature Review
The present discussion has already demonstrated the importance of occupational
knowledge in career decision-making. However, the question of interest is regarding the relation
between college students’ knowledge of their chosen occupations and their levels of certainty
with those choices. As career certainty is an important career decision-making outcome variable,
it has a large literature foundation. For that reason and because the population of interest in the
34
present discussion is specific, the literature related to college students will be reviewed. In
addition, it will be limited to studies addressing the topics of vocational interests, self-efficacy,
and occupational knowledge. A PsycInfo search of career with certainty or decidedness, and
college or undergraduate yielded 71 peer-reviewed articles written in English. Twenty-one of
the 71 articles examined career certainty with either interests, self-efficacy, or occupational
knowledge. Within this search, career certainty is predominantly measured by the Certainty
Scale of the Career Decision Scale (Osipow, Carney, & Barak, 1976), a two-item scale reflecting
the extent to which participants have made definite decisions about a career. Other measures
comprised of similar item content have been used as well (e.g., Career Decision Profile, Jones,
1989; Occupational Alternatives Question, Zener & Schnuelle, 1976).
Interests. Nine studies were found that looked at both career certainty and interests. Four
studies examined the relation between certainty and interest differentiation. Contrary to
Holland’s (1997) predictions regarding differentiation, none found evidence for the power of the
degree of differentiation among people’s interest profiles in predicting career certainty (Alvi,
Khan, & Kirkwood, 1990; Hirschi, 2009; Larson, Heppner, Ham, & Dugan, 1988; Sackett &
Hansen, 1995). Notably, the Hirschi and Sackett and Hansen studies failed to find significant
relations between these two constructs concurrently or longitudinally. These results suggest that
relative elevation of interests are not predictive of one’s certainty regarding his or her chosen
career. Two studies looked at interest consistency, which refers to degree of similarity between
interest types based on Holland’s hexagon model (1997). Larson and colleagues (1988) found no
differences between decided and undecided students in interest profile consistency; however,
deviation from Holland’s hexagon structure interests was found to be negatively related to career
35
certainty in a more recent study (Tracey & Darcy, 2002). Thus, the evidence regarding the
relation between interest consistency and career certainty is mixed.
Another construct of Holland’s is congruence, which refers to the degree of similarity
between one’s career choice and one’s vocational interests (Holland, 1997). Three studies were
found that examined congruence and they produced opposing results (Slaney & Slaney, 1986;
Tracey, 2008; Tracey, 2010). Slaney and Slaney (1986) found no differences in career certainty
between students whose interests were congruent with their career choices and students whose
interests were incongruent. However, Tracey (2008, 2010) found interest-occupation congruence
to be significantly related to career certainty in two separate samples (r = .29 and .26,
respectively). Similar to the research examining consistency and certainty, congruence has
yielded mixed findings. In addition to Holland’s constructs, one study was found that examined
the stability of interests over four to six weeks, finding that stability was unrelated to career
certainty (Berk & Fekken, 1990).
Career decision-making self-efficacy. Five studies were located that examined the
relation between career decision-making self-efficacy (CDMSE) and career certainty. CDMSE,
defined as one’s belief that he or she is able to successfully engage in and complete tasks
necessary to career decision-making (Betz, 2000), has been shown to relate moderately with
career certainty in four studies (r = .28 to .46; Creed & Patton, 2003; Restubog, Florentino, &
Garcia, 2010; Taylor & Popma, 1990; Tracey, 2008). Creed, Prideaux, and Patton (2005) found
that individuals who were certain about their future careers had significantly greater CDMSE
than their uncertain counterparts, F (1, 221) = 22.75, p < .001. In addition, Tracey (2008) found
that career certainty moderately relates to CDMSE concurrently, but not to CDMSE measured 13
months later at Time 2. However, Time 1 certainty significantly correlated with Time 2 certainty
36
(r = .25), which in turn correlated moderately with Time 2 CDMSE (r = .44). These results are
consistent with career decision-making models, as CDMSE is thought to increase one’s ability to
navigate the career decision-making process.
Occupational knowledge. Four studies were found that examined both occupational
knowledge and career certainty in college students. As previously reviewed in the occupational
knowledge section, small relations have been found between career certainty and self-estimated
levels of occupational knowledge (Multon et al., 1995; Westbrook et al., 1996). However,
studies examining objective occupational knowledge and career certainty have revealed null
relations with career certainty (Westbrook et al., 1996; Westbrook & Sloan, 2006). While none
of these findings are addressing knowledge specific to one’s chosen career, Westbrook and
colleagues (1996) participants did find a strong correlation between certainty and self-estimated
knowledge of the attributes required for participants’ chosen careers (r = .65, N = 91; e.g.,
“Know the duties required in career choice,” “Have information about career choice”). This
strong relation makes sense; greater knowledge of one’s career choice is associated with greater
career certainty. However, given that self-estimated knowledge was used, it remains unknown
whether participants’ self-estimations are accurate. Notably, this latter finding is the only one
addressing individuals’ knowledge of their chosen occupations rather than broad occupational
knowledge. This relation is of importance to the present discussion, especially since it produced
such a high correlation. The fact that participants in that study perceived their knowledge of their
career choices to be high when their certainty was high is an association one might expect based
on conceptual knowledge. This association could be interpreted such that greater knowledge of a
specific occupation leads one to be more certain that it is the correct decision, or greater certainty
37
about pursuing a certain career leads one to seek more knowledge about it. Either way, this
relation is expected based on the career decision-making models.
The fourth study examined occupational knowledge and certainty in more depth by
investigating college students majoring in secondary education who participated in a 40-hour
teacher observation course assignment (Thomas & Bruning, 1984). They found that 33 of the
224 participants dropped out of the secondary education major program after spending a week
observing a teacher in their subjects of interest. Before the observation, the non-dropout and
dropout groups had equal levels of career choice satisfaction and career certainty. At postintervention, the dropout group had decreased on all of those variables, effectively separating
themselves from the non-dropout group. Furthermore, the authors found significant differences
in the proportions of each group that had decided to major in secondary education within the past
year (52% for dropouts vs. 29% for non-dropouts) as opposed to more than one year ago.
While the size of the dropout group is quite small, especially compared to the sample as a
whole, the findings of this study are of great conceptual importance. The 33 dropout participants
had previously committed to the secondary education major; yet, after observing what teaching
actually entails, these students realized that they no longer desired to pursue a teaching degree. In
addition, 17 of those 33 dropout students had made a decision to major in education within the
past year, suggesting that the career may still have been new to them and their knowledge about
it may have been lacking. This study demonstrates an important point: there may be a portion of
college students who feel they are certain about their chosen careers, but are also uninformed
about the careers they are pursuing until they are forced to experience them first-hand in the real
world. Luckily, the students in this study had key exposure experiences early in their college
38
educations. Other students may not get this knowledge until they begin applying for jobs after
graduation.
An additional study was found that examined the role of job information on one’s intent
to remain in that job. Assessing employed sales agents at several different companies, Pitt and
Ramaseshan (1995) examined the extent to which participants retrospectively felt they received
realistic job information during their interviews now that they had actually been hired and were
experiencing the job first-hand. Participants indicated the amount, personal relevance, depth, and
accuracy of job information they received during their interviews. They also indicated the
frequency with which they currently considered quitting their recently obtained jobs. The authors
then compared those who “never/seldom” considered quitting their jobs and people who
“occasionally/often/all the time” considered quitting. Significant differences found on all
information indices, with people never/seldom considering quitting reporting lower amount,
relevance, depth, and accuracy of information. Further, a regression model including all
information indices accounted for 44% of the variance in quitting intentions. These results
suggest that those who were informed about the job they were committing to are the same
individuals who intended to remain in that job.
Translating these findings to college students, it may be that some students are not fully
informed regarding what they will be doing in their jobs on a daily basis. Being uninformed
could lead to dissatisfaction with jobs, intentions to quit, and possibly the desire to pursue a
different occupation altogether. The available literature suggests that students perceive
themselves to be certain to the extent they are knowledgeable about the careers they are
pursuing. However, due to the paucity of valid and reliable occupational knowledge assessment
methods, the relation between objectively measured occupational knowledge students have about
39
their chosen careers and career certainty remains unknown. Grotevant and Durrett (1980) were
able to provide evidence that some students’ estimates of knowledge about their chosen careers
was overestimated, at least with regards to one important aspect which was the level of education
needed to obtain their chosen occupations. When considered along with Westbrook and
colleagues’ (1996) .65 correlation between self-estimated knowledge of chosen careers and
certainty, this finding causes concern. If Grotevant and Durrett’s results are representative of
students’ judgments of their knowledge, then Westbrook and colleagues’ finding may be a gross
overestimation. If the true relation is closer to zero, which would be contrary to many
conceptually-based expectations, perhaps it is more complex and involves more variables than
previously thought.
Variables Predicting Career Certainty Alongside Occupational Knowledge
A mediator is a variable that explains at least part of the relation between two other
variables (Frazier, Tix, & Barron, 2004). In other words, a mediator is a mechanism through
which one variable influences another variable. Frazier and colleagues describe an example in
which a counseling intervention has an effect on well-being. However, when social support is
introduced to the relation, it is revealed that the intervention actually increases social support,
which, in turn, increases well-being. Thus, the relation between the intervention and well-being
is fully mediated by social support.
The present discussion has thus far argued, conceptually, that knowledge about one’s
chosen occupation should be positively related to one’s level of career certainty. However, no
literature has been found that directly supports this argument. This is partly due to measurement
methodology flaws; though it could also be due to confounding variables. In addition, the career
decision process may be more complex, leading to the equivocal nature of findings presented in
40
the literature. Therefore, four additional constructs found in the career decision-making literature
were examined for their potential roles in predicting career certainty alongside occupational
knowledge: career decision-making self-efficacy, interest congruence, career exploration
activities, and perceived occupational knowledge.
Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy
Self-efficacy, theorized as one’s perception of his or her probable effectiveness in a given
situation in the near future (Bandura, 1977), has received significant attention in the career
development literature due to its broad applicability to both the content and process domains of
career decision-making. Based on Bandura’s (1977, 1986, 1997) work, self-efficacy is grounded
in social cognitive theory and is a mechanism determining one’s response to new or threatening
situations. Bandura and others (e.g., Betz, 2004; Betz & Hackett, 1981; Lent, Brown, & Hackett,
1994) posit that higher self-efficacy leads individuals to approach new or threatening situations
with the belief that they will be able to cope successfully. Conversely, lower self-efficacy may
result in avoidant behavior leading to preference for the familiar and safe instead of the new and
uncertain.
Specific to the career decision process, career decision-making self-efficacy has been the
focus of a substantial amount of research. This term refers to one’s belief that he or she is able to
successfully engage in and complete tasks necessary to career decision-making (Betz, 2000). The
Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale (CDSES; Taylor & Betz, 1983), previously named the
Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy Scale, is the most well-established measure of this
construct. Anchored in Crites’ (1976) five career choice competencies, the short and long forms
of the CDSES include five subscales: self-appraisal, occupational information, goal selection,
planning, and problem solving. These subscales key in to the important domains of career
41
development that require action on the part of the individual. In other words, these areas
comprise the different stages of information gathering and implementation needed to facilitate
mature career development.
Career certainty. As reviewed previously, career decision-making self-efficacy has not
been frequently examined in the literature with career certainty. The five studies that did look at
this relation utilized the CDSES, with three studies finding that it correlated moderately with
career certainty (r = .38 to .46; Creed & Patton, 2003; Restubog et al., 2010; Taylor & Popma,
1990). The latter authors also examined this relation in terms of the CDSES subscales, finding
significant correlations between career certainty and one’s perceived ability to assess his or her
vocational interests, values, and abilities (r = .43); obtain occupational information (r = .32);
select career goals (r = .55); make plans to pursue a career (r = .46); and solve career-related
problems (r = .30). Creed, Prideaux, and Patton (2005) found that individuals who were certain
about their future careers had significantly greater CDMSE than their uncertain counterparts, F
(1, 221) = 22.75, p < .001. In addition, Tracey (2008) found career certainty moderately related
to the CDSES concurrently (r = .41), but not to the CDSES measured 13 months later at Time 2
(r = .13). However, Time 1 certainty had a small to moderate relation to Time 2 certainty (r =
.25), which in turn correlated moderately with Time 2 CDSES (r = .44). These results are
consistent with career decision-making theories, as career decision-making self-efficacy is
conceptually thought to increase one’s ability to navigate the career decision-making process.
Occupational knowledge. Career decision-making self-efficacy has thus far not been
examined in relation to occupational knowledge. Two studies have measured “career
knowledge” and career decision-making self-efficacy measured by the CDSES, but career
knowledge in both cases was operationally defined as knowledge of decision-making skills and
42
world of work information rather than as knowledge of specific aspects of occupations (Creed &
Patton, 2003; Creed, Patton, & Prideaux, 2007). However, given that the CDSES – Occupational
Information scale measures one’s belief in his or her ability to obtain information about
occupations, it is possible that a relation between the CDSES and occupational knowledge could
exist. For example, as reviewed previously, occupational knowledge has been shown to
moderately relate to academic achievement and cognitive ability in several studies. Additionally,
academic performance measured by GPA has been shown to be related to scores on the CDSES
(r = .20; Restubog et al., 2010). It is possible that a spurious relationship between the CDSES
and occupational knowledge could account for some of the variance in GPA’s relationship with
the two variables separately.
Career decision-making self-efficacy, occupational knowledge, and career certainty.
Career decision-making self-efficacy has been actively researched over the past thirty years,
predominantly due to the development of the Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale by Taylor and
Betz in 1983. However, it has not yet been examined alongside both objective occupational
knowledge and career certainty. Therefore, the present study sought to investigate whether career
decision-making self-efficacy, measured by the CDSES, has a significant relation with
participants’ objective occupational knowledge of their chosen careers. In addition, the interplay
between career decision-making self-efficacy and objective occupational knowledge will be
examined in terms of their roles in predicting career certainty. A mediation effect was predicted,
such that career decision-making self-efficacy will relate to career certainty both directly and
indirectly, through objective occupational knowledge.
The relations between these three variables is important to understand because college
students’ career certainty, according to career decision-making theories (Hirschi & Lage, 2007),
43
should be influenced largely by knowledge of the occupations they are considering. However,
career decision-making self-efficacy may be involved by providing some students with greater
senses of contentment with their perceived abilities to make career decision, or greater selfefficacy regarding their abilities to seek out further occupational knowledge. Conversely, career
decision-making self-efficacy may impede students’ abilities to make career decisions with
which they are comfortable, or leave them paralyzed in the career decision process by preventing
them from seeking occupational knowledge necessary for making decisions. In these ways,
objective occupational knowledge may partially mediate the relation between career decisionmaking self-efficacy and career certainty.
Vocational Interest
Vocational interests describe individuals’ preferences regarding work activities,
situations, and environments (Su, Rounds, & Armstrong, 2009), and have been established as
predictors of college students’ academic majors (e.g., Gasser, Larson, & Borgen, 2007). This
construct has received substantial attention in the literature, with a large part dedicated to John
Holland’s (1997) work on one of the most well-known theories of vocational interests. He
identified six factors that largely encompass the types of interests people have regarding work,
referred to as the RIASEC model (Realistic: constructing, mechanical, outdoor interests;
Investigative: inquiry and discovery; Artistic: creating, designing; Social: serving, helping
others; Enterprising: persuading others; Conventional: organizing and structuring things/data).
This six-domain model has emerged as the dominant model vocational psychology employs to
organize vocational interests. Part of the utility of Holland’s model is its application to both
people’s interests and their work environments. For both people and environments, interest
profiles can be created based on the degree to which each factor fits with the individual or
44
environment. The order of the three highest scores when the six domains are measured comprises
one’s “Holland Code” (e.g., “IAS” for investigative, artistic, and social interests), with the first
letter representing the interest type with the highest score. These codes are computed for people
based on their responses to interest inventories, while work environments’ codes are computed
based on the types of activities involved in occupations and are similarly organized around the
RIASEC domains (Spokane & Cruza-Guet, 2005).
Person-environment fit. Holland’s RIASEC model fits within a larger vocational
outcome variable, person-environment fit (PE fit). PE fit is the extent to which a person’s
interests and his or her current or prospective work environment are compatible (Dawis, 2005).
The general understanding is that no person’s interests will fit perfectly with the activities in his
or her work environment; however, the goal is for individuals to decide which career path will
likely be most compatible with their vocational interests. Career indecision can arise when
people have difficulty finding an acceptable fit, which is a realization that can occur anywhere
from the initial stages of the decision process to years after one has committed to a career and
entered the workforce.
Interest Congruence. Person-Environment fit can be assessed in terms of a number of
vocational variables. Regarding interests however, Holland’s RIASEC model is commonly used
to measure PE fit. Holland (1997) described “congruence” as an indicator of PE fit, typically
referring to congruence between and individual’s interest and his or her occupation. Specifically,
congruence is the degree to which a person’s Holland code matches that of a given work
environment (Spokane & Cruza-Guet, 2005).
Congruence was hypothesized by Holland to predict several positive vocational
outcomes, including one’s job satisfaction. The importance of assessing the ability of congruence
45
to predict job satisfaction comes from the fact that, for many individuals, a career constitutes a
substantial portion of life; people tend to desire careers with which they will be satisfied.
However, the literature in support of Holland’s congruence-job satisfaction hypothesis is
somewhat mixed. Between congruence and job satisfaction, an overall mean correlation of .20
was found across 17 studies meta-analyzed by Tranberg and colleagues (1993). Mean
correlations ranged from .12 and .45 depending on the type of congruence index used. Overall,
Tranberg and colleagues found no significant effect of congruence on job satisfaction,
concluding that it cannot predict satisfaction alone. A more recent meta-analysis again
examining the congruence-job satisfaction relation produced similar findings (Tsabari, Tziner, &
Mier, 2005). They found mean congruence-job satisfaction correlations across 36 samples to
range from .07 to .35 depending on the congruence index used.
The authors of both meta-analyses were compelled to conclude that the congruence-job
satisfaction relation was not well-supported in the literature. However, this finding was due in
part to the heterogenous nature of congruence indices utilized in research. While various
congruence indices exist and most have both advantages and disadvantages, the profile
correlation (Q correlation; Cronbach & Gleser, 1953) has been identified as one of the
statistically stronger methods of measuring congruence (Armstrong et al., 2010). It utilizes
participants’ and occupations’ full interest profiles (i.e., all 6 RIASEC scores) in order to get the
most comprehensive estimate of congruence. Measuring the extent to which two interest profiles
have similar shapes, the Q correlation is also consistent with Holland’s (1997) original
conceptualization of congruence (Armstrong et al., 2010).
The Euclidean distance index, another method of measuring congruence, has been argued
as the best measure of interest profile similarity as it describes elevation differences in addition
46
to shape differences (Armstrong et al., 2010; Cronbach & Gleser, 1953). However, the present
study is concerned only about the degree to which participants’ highest interests match those of
their chosen occupations. In other words, relative interest elevation is important rather than
absolute elevation. Thus, in the context of the present study the Q correlation is supported as an
appropriate measurement of congruence.
Career certainty. While the discussion of congruence and job satisfaction is more
applicable to currently employed adults, this relation can be prospectively assessed in college
students by examining more proximal, related constructs. Congruence has been found to relate to
career certainty (e.g., Tracey, 2010), which in turn has been related to college students’ academic
major satisfaction (e.g., Ware & Pogge, 1980). Career certainty has also been associated with
persistence in academic major programs 18 months later (Restubog et al., 2010), a construct
considered to be conceptually related to major satisfaction (Nauta, 2007). Persistence measured
in a sample of working adults also revealed that individuals who were persisting in their current
jobs, compared to those who were changing careers, had higher levels of congruence after
controlling for age and career tenure (Donohue, 2006). Overall, the available literature does
provide support for the congruence-career certainty relation, both directly and indirectly.
Occupational knowledge. Few studies have examined occupational knowledge in
relation to congruence. Significant differences between individuals with and without congruent
career choices (measured based on the KPR interest scales; Roe, 1956) were found on
objectively measured broad occupational knowledge and self-estimated knowledge of chosen
careers’ required education and attributes (Westbrook & Parry-Hill, 1973). These findings
suggest that greater knowledge of common occupations is related to one’s identification of
congruent career paths, and that congruence may contribute to one’s self-estimated knowledge of
47
aspects of his or her chosen career. In a more specific study, Van Iddekinge et al. (2011) found
that levels of Realistic interests, a domain congruent with the Army work environment, are
associated with Army-related job task knowledge. Knowledge in this study was objectively
measured, indicating that greater interest in a given field may lead one to seek out and acquire
more knowledge of activities in that field. Notably, however, this study examined a relatively
unique sample of individuals and the results may not generalize to general college student
samples.
Interest congruence as a mediator. The theory behind interest congruence suggests
individuals naturally seek out work environments they believe will be compatible with their own
interests, leading them toward occupational satisfaction (Holland, 1997). The literature has
provided moderate support for this theory (Tranberg, Slane, & Ekeberg, 1993); though, it is
important to note that assessing one’s congruence with his or her chosen occupation is an
internal judgment. Individuals must assess their own levels of congruence as they navigate
through the career decision process. Conceptually, one would expect increasing occupational
knowledge to Help individuals in assessing which career will yield the highest congruence.
Therefore, if greater knowledge leads to greater likelihood of achieving interest congruence, one
might expect level of career certainty to positively relate to both objective occupational
knowledge and interest congruence. In other words, interest congruence may act as a partial
mediator between objective occupational knowledge and career certainty, so objective
occupational knowledge would have both a direct effect on career certainty, and an indirect
effect through interest congruence.
48
Career Exploration Activities
Career exploration activities are specific ways of gaining occupational knowledge, both
broadly and in-depth, which comprise an important career exploration phase of most career
decision-making models (Hirschi & Lage, 2007). The present study operationalized this
construct by creating an checklist-style inventory to gauge the types of career exploration
activities in which participants have already engaged (Appendix G). The ability to engage in
these activities has been deemed a “fundamental competence” for individuals immersed in the
career decision-making process (Blustein, 1997).
The present study examined this construct in relation to career decision-making selfefficacy and objective occupational knowledge. The Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale (Taylor
& Betz, 1983) includes ten items gauging participants’ self-efficacy related to actively seeking
occupational knowledge through various common career exploration activities. These activities,
in turn, are central to gaining occupational knowledge. Therefore, number of reported career
exploration activities was examined as a potential mediator between career decision-making selfefficacy and objective occupational knowledge. Specifically, the relation between career
decision-making self-efficacy and objective occupational knowledge was hypothesized to be
fully mediated by career exploration activities. In other words, career decision-making selfefficacy was thought to influence engagement in career exploration activities, which would then
influence amount of occupational knowledge gained.
Perceived Occupational Knowledge
This construct was added to the model predicting career certainty in part because of its
prominent role in previous studies measuring “occupational knowledge.” Perceived, or selfestimated, occupational knowledge has been frequently employed as a proxy to examining
49
objectively measured knowledge of participants’ chosen occupations. While its use is
understandable given the inherent difficulty in measuring this construct across large participant
samples, it remains unknown whether college students’ perceptions of their occupational
knowledge are accurate. Thus, the present study included perceived occupational knowledge for
the primary purpose of determining whether it is a sufficient substitution for the difficult
objective measurement of occupational knowledge. In addition, perceived occupational
knowledge was thought to mediate the relation between career decision-making self-efficacy and
career certainty. While career decision-making self-efficacy measures students’ beliefs about
their future effectiveness in navigating career decision-making tasks, perceived occupational
knowledge forces students to specifically gauge the amount of occupational knowledge they
have already obtained. Therefore, career decision-making self-efficacy may influence students’
perceptions of their occupational knowledge, which in turn may influence whether students
believe they are certain about the careers they have chosen. In this way, perceived occupational
knowledge may act as a mediator in the relation between career decision-making self-efficacy
and career certainty.
50
CHAPTER THREE
METHODS
Design
The present study utilized a descriptive correlational design as it allowed for assessment
of statistical relations between variables within participants. The primary variables were
objective occupational knowledge and career certainty. Career decision-making self-efficacy,
interest congruence, reported career exploration activities, and perceived occupational
knowledge were also examined alongside objective occupational knowledge in predicting career
certainty.
Participants
The target population of the present study was undergraduate college students. Data were
collected in the fall semester of 2013 from undergraduate students in introductory psychology
courses at Iowa State University. Data from 357 participants were originally collected. Forty-one
participants’ data were excluded prior to analysis for various reasons. As a single career-of-focus
was needed for analysis of the Work Activities Profile, 21 were excluded because they identified
a) no careers of focus, b) more than one career of focus (e.g., “a sales representative or a coach”),
c) ambiguous or invalid careers (e.g., “something in the business field”), d) careers for which
O*NET does not yet have data (e.g., “military officer”), or e) if the career they indicated at the
beginning of the survey did not match the career they used to fill out the Work Activity
Importance Profile items. Additional exclusions included two participants who reported being
under 18 years of age, and 18 participants due to duplicate or incomplete survey responses.
The final sample consisted of 316 participants. Two hundred twenty-seven participants
identified as female (71.8%) and 89 identified as male (28.2%). Participant ages ranged from 18
51
to 67 years (M = 19.36, SD = 3.71); one participant did not indicate age. One hundred seventysix participants were first-year students (55.7%), 75 were second-year students (23.7%), 39 were
third-year students (12.3%), 13 were fourth-year students (4.1%), 10 were fifth-year students
(3.2%), 2 were sixth-year students (0.6%), and 1 was a graduate students (0.3%). Two hundred
thirty-seven participants had declared their majors at the time of the study (75%) and 79 had not
(25%). Regarding the ethnic diversity of the sample, 254 identified as European American
(80.4%), 15 as Pacific Islander/Asian American (4.7%), 4 as African American (1.3%), 8 as
Latino/a American (2.5%), 7 as multiracial American (2.2%), and 28 as international students
(8.9%).
Measures
Objective occupational knowledge. Objective occupational knowledge was measured
by the Work Activity Importance Profile (WAIP), adapted for the present study based on
O*NET’s Detailed Work Activity database (National Center for O*NET Development, 2003).
The database includes a set of 41 work activities (Appendix B) rated by importance for each of
the 974 O*NET occupational titles. Average importance ratings for each activity are scored on a
scale from (0) not important to (100) extremely important for each occupation based on data
collected from occupation experts and random samples of workers (Research Triangle Institute,
2006). For each item, respondents answered the question, “How important is the activity to your
current job?” Respondents’ answers were then averaged for each item to create the importance
score for each work activity within a given occupation. The 41 work activities are the same
across 974 occupations in the database which allows for practical, efficient comparison of
occupational knowledge in large research samples. As an example, a portion of the occupation
activity importance profile for an accountant is presented in Appendix C.
52
To measure participants’ objective occupational knowledge in the present study, they
were asked to indicate the degree to which each of the 41 activities of the WAIP is important
with regards to the career choice each participant listed at the beginning of the survey. Eight
activities were seen on a page at a time. Participants responded on a 5-point Likert scale ranging
from (1) not important to (5) extremely important. The coefficient alpha for this scale in the
present study was .93. Additional reliability statistics were unavailable as this was the first study
to utilize these items in this manner.
Accuracy of occupational knowledge. A profile correlation (Q correlation; Cronbach &
Gleser, 1953) was computed for each participant to produce occupational knowledge scores. The
Q correlation, with scores ranging from -1.00 to +1.00, provided a standardized measurement of
the agreement between a participant’s WAIP ratings for his or her chosen occupation and the
corresponding O*NET work activity rating profile for the same occupation. A score of .00 would
indicate no pattern of similarity between profiles, scores nearing +1.00 would indicate greater
profile matches, and scores reaching -1.00 would indicate nearly opposite profiles (Armstrong et
al., 2010). This statistic is not descriptive of elevation differences between two profiles, but of
differences in shape. Since the importance scale is not descriptive in and of itself, participants’
knowledge was not assessed based on their ratings of the importance of a given activity, but
rather on the importance they assigned a given activity relative to all other activities. Computed
in this way, a score of +1.00 indicates the highest level of occupational knowledge, and as scores
move further away from +1.00 they become associated with lower occupational knowledge.
Perceived occupational knowledge. A subjective measure of occupational knowledge,
the Perceived Occupational Knowledge Scale developed by the author (Pesch, 2014a; Appendix
D), was also administered. Each participant was asked to estimate the knowledge he or she has
53
about the activities involved in his or her chosen career. The 3-item scale created by the author
was used to assess self-estimated knowledge on a 6-point scale from (1) strongly disagree to (6)
strongly agree with one reverse-scored item. Mean scores were computed. This measure
provided information about the relation between objectively- and subjectively-measured
occupational knowledge. The coefficient alpha for this scale in the present study was .73.
Career certainty. Career certainty was measured using the Career Certainty Scale
(CCS), a 3-item scale adapted from the Career Decision Scale (Osipow et al., 1976; Appendix
E). This scale assessed the extent to which participants were certain about pursuing the career
they indicated as their current career of choice. Response options were on a 6-point scale from
(1) strongly disagree to (6) strongly agree. Mean scores were computed. The coefficient alpha
for this scale in the present study was .92. The 2-item original version of this scale (Osipow et
al., 1976) has been established with internal consistency estimates between .80 and .85
(Hartman, Fuqua, & Hartman, 1983; Tracey, 2008). Osipow (1980) reported a two-week testretest correlation of .82, and its validity has been well-established (Betz, 1992; Osipow, 1980).
Career decision-making self-efficacy. In order to assess for career decision-making
self-efficacy, the present study employed the Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale (CDSES;
Taylor & Betz, 1983; Appendix F). The CDSES is among the most well-established measures of
this construct. It was designed to measure one’s belief that he or she will be able to successfully
engage in and complete tasks necessary to career decision-making (Betz, 2000). Anchored in
Crites’ (1976) five career choice competencies, the short and long forms of the CDSES include
five subscales: self-appraisal, occupational information, goal selection, planning, and problem
solving. Participants responded by indicating their perceived abilities to complete career
decision-making tasks on a 5-point Likert scale from (1) no confidence at all to (5) complete
54
confidence. Mean scores were used, with higher scores reflecting stronger beliefs regarding one’s
probable effectiveness in career decision-making tasks.
Internal consistency has been established for the 5-level continuum scoring method of the
CDSES (α = .78 to .87) and for the total score (α = .93 to .95; Betz, Hammond, & Multon, 2005;
Betz & Voyten, 1997; Betz, Klein, & Taylor, 1996). Moderate estimates of concurrent validity
with vocational identity have been shown (.40 to .66; Betz et al., 1996). Meta-analytic findings
have also provided evidence of concurrent validity with self-esteem (mean r = .49) and career
indecision (mean r = -.52; Choi et al., 2012), which refers to a general inability to make careerrelated decisions.
In the present study, the CDSES subscales were found to be highly related with all
correlations above .74. Therefore, the whole CDSES mean score was used in analyses. The
coefficient alpha for this scale was .96.
Career exploration activities. The Career Exploration Checklist, developed by the
author (Pesch, 2014b), was used to assess the extent to which participants have engaged in career
exploration activities. Items were derived from career exploration activities described in the
Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale (Taylor & Betz, 1983), career decision-making models
(Crites, 1976; Gati, 1986; Harren, 1979, Hirschi & Lage, 2007), and Iowa State University’s
Career Exploration Services program. This 17-item checklist (Appendix G) asked each
participant to indicate the extent to which he or she has engaged in the various exploration
activities related to his or her chosen career. Responses were recorded on a 3-point scale, (0)
never, (1) once, and (2) more than once. Sum scores were computed. The present study utilized
this measure to assess the level of correspondence between participants’ career decision-making
55
self-efficacy beliefs specific to career exploration activities, and actual engagement in those
activities. The coefficient alpha for this scale was .84.
Vocational interests. The O*NET Interest Profiler – Short Form (IP; Rounds, Su, Lewis,
& Rivkin, 2010; Appendix H) is a public domain 60-item instrument designed to measure
Holland’s (1997) six types of occupational interests (Realistic, Investigative, Artistic, Social,
Enterprising, and Conventional). Each of Holland’s interest types comprises its own 10-item
subscale. Each item presented an activity and participants responded by indicating their
preference for the activity on a 5-point scale from (0) strongly dislike to (4) strongly like.
Summed scores were calculated for each subscale with higher scores indicating more interest in a
given interest category. Rounds and colleagues reported internal consistency estimates ranging
from .78 to .90, and test-retest reliability estimates ranging from .78 to .86. Furthermore,
convergent and discriminant validity evidence was found with the RIASEC scales of the InterestFinder (Wall & Baker, 1997). Correlations for same-named subscales ranged from .74 to .82, and
from .12 to .48 for differing subscales. The coefficient alpha for this scale in the present study
was .93. The O*NET Interest Profiler – Short Form was used by the present study in tandem
with the Occupational Interest Profiles for O*NET to construct a measure of interest congruence
(both described below).
Occupational interest profiles. Interest profiles for occupations were determined using
the Occupational Interest Profiles for O*NET (OIP; Rounds, Smith, Hubert, Lewis, & Rivkin,
1999). As part of a larger occupational information database, occupational interest profiles were
created. The profiles are based on Holland’s RIASEC model (1997), and thus comparable to the
person interest profiling instruments. Each of O*NET’s 1172 occupational titles is paired with a
RIASEC interest profile, with unique values for each interest type based on the occupation’s
56
characteristics. Job incumbent interest profiles, discriminant function analysis, and expert
judgments were used in determining interest profiles for each occupation. This system, created
for organizing and describing occupations, has replaced the Dictionary of Occupational Titles for
the U.S. Department of Labor and is currently the only comprehensive system available.
Evidence for external and structural validity is presented in O*NET’s developmental report
(Rounds et al., 1999).
Interest congruence. Interest congruence scores were computed using the O*NET
Interest Profiler – Short Form for participants’ interest scores and the Occupational Interest
Profiles for O*NET (both described above). Congruence is the degree to which a person’s
Holland code matches that of a given work environment (Spokane & Cruza-Guet, 2005). In the
present study, congruence was illustrated using the Q correlation (Cronbach & Gleser, 1953)
which is a correlation of two response profiles. Each participant’s interest profile across the
RIASEC, measured by the IP, was correlated with the O*NET occupation interest profile of his
or her chosen career. In the context of the present study, the Q correlation was supported as an
appropriate measurement of congruence (Armstrong et al., 2010). It is consistent with Holland’s
(1997) original conceptualization of congruence and measures the extent to which two interest
profiles have similar shape. Validity for less stringent, but more frequently used congruence
calculations (e.g., 1st letter agreement, 2-letter agreement) are presented in the meta-analysis by
Tranberg and colleagues (1993). The specific congruence calculation method used in the present
study has not yet been used in the literature, but is more comprehensive; comprising scores on all
six interest categories for both participants and careers.
57
Procedure
Approval by the Institutional Review Board at Iowa State University was obtained prior
to data collection in order to ensure the present study is in compliance with the ethical standards
defined by the American Psychological Association.
Participants were recruited using the Department of Psychology’s online research
participation system that moderates undergraduate students’ participation in departmentassociated research projects. The present study was one of many web-based survey options in
which students were able to participate. Qualtrics was used to create the survey and collect
responses. Undergraduate students who opted to partake in the survey received one credit for
participating. Prior to their participation in the survey, students were presented with an informed
consent statement containing a brief description of the study’s purpose and procedures
(Appendix I). They indicated their informed consent in order to begin the survey, though they
were informed of their right to discontinue study participation at any time without forfeiting
participation credit.
Students were first presented with demographic questions that included items asking each
participant to provide the career he or she is pursuing or seriously considering pursuing.
Subsequent pages of the survey presented participants with the Perceived Occupational
Knowledge Scale, Career Certainty Scale, Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale, Career
Exploration Checklist, O*NET Interest Profiler – Short Form, and the Work Activity Importance
Profile. Upon reaching the end of the survey, participants were debriefed regarding the purpose
of the study (Appendix J). The researcher’s contact information was provided if participants had
questions or concerns regarding the study.
58
CHAPTER FOUR
RESULTS
Descriptive Statistics and Preliminary Analyses
SPSS 20 and Mplus Version 6.0 (Muthén & Muthén, 2010) were used for data analysis.
Fisher z prime (z`) transformations were conducted on two variables before running descriptive
statistics and preliminary analyses (Cohen et al., 2003). This transformation was statistically
appropriate for the occupational knowledge and interest congruence Q score distributions, given
they ranged from -1.00 to +1.00. The resulting distributions for those two variables are presented
as z-scores; they were used throughout data analysis in place of the original Q scores. To
calculate z-scores, the following equation was used: z` = ½ (ln(1+r)-ln(1-r)), where r represents
each individual’s Q score for the respective variable. This transformation adjusts the
distributions to make them symmetrical and normalize the standard error, allowing for more
accurate confidence intervals when estimating standardized beta values in path analysis (Cohen
et al., 2003). The ranges for objective occupational knowledge and interest congruence remain
between -1.00 and +1.00, with scores near zero indicating no relation between participant and
occupation profiles, and scores near +1.00 indicating greater similarity between the two profiles.
Means, standard deviations, coefficient alphas, and zero-order Pearson product moment
correlations among the six variables are presented in Table 1. A comparison between the present
study’s sample statistics for the Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale was compared to that of
Tracey’s (2008) sample, comprised of a similar distribution of first- through fourth-year college
students. The present study’s sample (N = 316, M = 3.72, SD = 0.54) was statistically different
from Tracey’s Time 1 data using the Short Form of the scale (N = 283, M = 3.40, SD = 0.96; t =
5.09, p < .001), but not statistically different from Tracey’s Time 2 data collected three months
59
later (N = 283, M = 3.82, SD = 1.07; t = 1.47, p = .143). This was the only comparable sample
since all other studies utilized high school samples or the 10-point response scale of the Career
Decision Self-Efficacy Scale.
Career decision-making self-efficacy positively correlated with career certainty (r = .47,
p < .001), reported number of career exploration behaviors (r = .42, p < .001), and perceived
occupational knowledge (r = .39, p < .001). All of these relations were medium effect sizes
(Cohen, 1992). Career certainty also positively correlated with career exploration behaviors (r =
.28, p < .001) , but did not significantly correlate with interest congruence (p > .05). Objective
occupational knowledge correlated significantly with interest congruence (r = .20, p < .001).
Although the effect was small, this correlation provides some support for the validity of the
Work Activity Importance Profile as a measure of occupational knowledge in this sample.
Objective occupational knowledge did not significantly correlate with career certainty, perceived
occupational knowledge, reported number of career exploration activities, or career decisionmaking self-efficacy (ps > .05). Correlations and descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1.
60
Table 1.
Descriptive Statistics and Variable Correlations (N = 316)
*Indicates significance at p ≤ .001
a
For all constructs, higher values indicate higher levels of the construct.
Note. Career decision-making self-efficacy as measured by the Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy Scale (Taylor
& Betz, 1983); career exploration activities as measured by the Career Exploration Checklist (Pesch, 2014b);
objective occupational knowledge as measured by the Work Activity Importance Profile (adapted from National
Center for O*NET Development, 2003); interest congruence as measured by the O*NET Interest Profiler (Rounds et
al., 2010); perceived occupational knowledge as measured by the Perceived Occupational Knowledge Scale (Pesch,
2014a); career certainty, as measured by the Career Certainty Scale (adapted from Osipow et al., 1976).
Variablea
1 2 3 4 5 6
1 Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy 1.00 .42* .06 .04 .39* .47*
2 Career Exploration Activities 1.00 .00 .00 .41* .28*
3 Objective Occupational Knowledge 1.00 .20* .06 -.02
4 Interest Congruence 1.00 .08 .10
5 Perceived Occupational Knowledge 1.00 .39*
6 Career Certainty 1.00
Mean 3.72 15.55 0.65 0.63 4.48 4.72
Standard Deviation 0.54 6.97 0.32 0.58 1.06 1.06
Coefficient Alpha .96 .84 .93 .93 .73 .92
61
Hypotheses
The four hypotheses were tested through multiple regression based on the mediation
testing model presented by Baron and Kenny (1986). They identified three conditions that must
be met in order for a variable to function as a mediator: 1) the independent variable must
significantly relate to the hypothesized mediator (Path A), 2) the hypothesize mediator must
significantly relate to the dependent variable (Path B), and 3) the previously significant relation
between the independent and dependent variables significantly decreases (or becomes null) when
Paths A and B are controlled. Three-step multiple regression models were run to test these
mediation hypotheses. Results are described below and presented in Table 2.
62
Table 2.
Multiple Regression Results for Mediation Hypotheses 1 – 4
Note. CDMSE = Career decision-making self-efficacy as measured by the Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy
Scale (Taylor & Betz, 1983); CEA = Career exploration activities as measured by the Career Exploration Checklist
(Pesch, 2014b); OOK = Objective occupational knowledge as measured by the Work Activity Importance Profile
(adapted from National Center for O*NET Development, 2003); IC = Interest congruence as measured by the
O*NET Interest Profiler (Rounds et al., 2010); POK = Perceived occupational knowledge as measured by the
Perceived Occupational Knowledge Scale (Pesch, 2014a); CC = Career certainty, as measured by the Career
Certainty Scale (adapted from Osipow et al., 1976).
Step Predictor(s) Criterion β t p
Hypothesis 1
1 CDMSE OOK .064 1.14 .256
2 CDMSE CC .466 9.35 < .001
3
CDMSE
CC
.470 9.39 < .001
OOK -.049 -0.97 .331
Hypothesis 2
1 CDMSE CEA .419 8.18 < .001
2 CDMSE OOK .064 1.14 .256
3
CDMSE
OOK
.080 1.29 .199
CEA -.038 -0.61 .544
Hypothesis 3
1 OOK IC .202 3.65 < .001
2 OOK CC -.019 -0.33 .742
3
OOK
CC
-.040 -0.70 .482
IC .108 1.88 .061
Hypothesis 4
1 CDMSE POK .405 7.86 < .001
2 CDMSE CC .466 9.35 < .001
3
CDMSE
CC
.371 7.00 < .001
POK .234 4.42 < .001
63
.47*
.06 -.05
Hypothesis 1 predicted objective occupational knowledge would partially mediate the
relation between career decision-making self-efficacy and career certainty (Figure 2). In Step 1,
multiple regression results revealed a non-significant effect of career decision-making selfefficacy on objective occupational knowledge, and Step 2 revealed a significant effect of career
decision-making self-efficacy on career certainty (β = .466, p < .001). Step 3 included both
career decision-making self-efficacy and objective occupational knowledge in the prediction of
career certainty. No significant changes occurred in this step; career decision-making selfefficacy had a significant effect on career certainty (β = .470, p < .001), while objective
occupational knowledge did not (see Table 2). This hypothesis was not supported by the data.
Instead, a significant relation existed between career decision-making self-efficacy and career
certainty after all other variables have been accounted for in the model; objective occupational
knowledge does not significantly relate to either variable. Like the figure
Figure 2. Hypothesis 1 mediation model with multiple regression results.
*Indicates significance at p < .001
Note: Solid lines indicate significant paths; dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Career decision-making selfefficacy as measured by the Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy Scale (Taylor & Betz, 1983); Objective
occupational knowledge as measured by the Work Activity Importance Profile (adapted from National Center for
O*NET Development, 2003); Career certainty, as measured by the Career Certainty Scale (adapted from Osipow et
al., 1976).
Hypothesis 2 predicted career exploration activities would partially mediate the relation
between career decision-making self-efficacy and objective occupational knowledge (Figure 3).
Career decisionmaking self-efficacy Career certainty
Objective
occupational
knowledge
64
In Step 1, multiple regression results revealed a significant effect of career decision-making selfefficacy on career exploration activities (β = .419, p < .001), and Step 2 revealed a nonsignificant effect of career decision-making self-efficacy on objective occupational knowledge.
Step 3 included both career decision-making self-efficacy and career exploration activities in the
prediction of objective occupational knowledge. No significant changes occurred in this step;
neither career decision-making self-efficacy nor career exploration activities had a significant
effect on objective occupational knowledge (see Table 2).
Figure 3. Hypothesis 2 mediation model with multiple regression results.
*Indicates significance at p < .001
Note: Solid lines indicate significant paths; dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Career decision-making selfefficacy as measured by the Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy Scale (Taylor & Betz, 1983); Objective
occupational knowledge as measured by the Work Activity Importance Profile (adapted from National Center for
O*NET Development, 2003); Career exploration activities as measured by the Career Exploration Checklist (Pesch,
2014b).
Hypotheses 2 was not supported by the data. Results indicated career exploration
activities did not mediate the relation between career decision-making self-efficacy and objective
occupational knowledge as predicted. Objective occupational knowledge did not significantly
relate to either variable; though career decision-making self-efficacy did have a significant direct
effect on career exploration activities. Neither mediation hypothesis was supported.
Hypothesis 3 predicted interest congruence would partially mediate the relation between
objective occupational knowledge and career certainty (Figure 4). In Step 1, multiple regression
results revealed a significant effect of objective occupational knowledge on interest congruence
.42* -.04
Career decisionmaking self-efficacy
Objective occupational
knowledge
Career
exploration
activities
.08
65
(β = .202, p < .001), and Step 2 revealed a non-significant effect of objective occupational
knowledge on career certainty. Step 3 included both objective occupational knowledge and
interest congruence in the prediction of career certainty. No significant changes occurred in this
step; neither objective occupational knowledge nor interest congruence had a significant effect
on career certainty (see Table 2). This partial mediation hypothesis was not supported by the
data. Results indicated objective occupational knowledge and interest congruence were
significantly related; though neither had a significant relation with career certainty.
Figure 4. Hypothesis 3 mediation model with multiple regression results.
*Indicates significance at p < .001
Note: Solid lines indicate significant paths; dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Objective occupational
knowledge as measured by the Work Activity Importance Profile (adapted from National Center for O*NET
Development, 2003); Interest congruence as measured by the O*NET Interest Profiler (Rounds et al., 2010); Career
certainty, as measured by the Career Certainty Scale (adapted from Osipow et al., 1976).
Hypothesis 4 predicted perceived occupational knowledge would partially mediate the
relation between career decision-making self-efficacy and career certainty (Figure 5). In Step 1,
multiple regression results revealed a significant effect of career decision-making self-efficacy
on perceived occupational knowledge (β = .405, p < .001), and Step 2 revealed a significant
effect of career decision-making self-efficacy on career certainty (β = .466, p < .001). Step 3
included both career decision-making self-efficacy and perceived occupational knowledge in the
prediction of career certainty. Significant effects of both career decision-making self-efficacy (β
= .371, p < .001) and perceived occupational knowledge (β = .234, p < .001) on career certainty
.20* .12
Interest
congruence
Objective occupational
knowledge
Career certainty -.04
66
remained (see Table 2). While the effect of career decision-making self-efficacy decreased with
the addition of perceived occupational knowledge, the change was not significant; results did not
support this partial mediation hypothesis.
Figure 5. Hypothesis 4 mediation model with multiple regression results.
*Indicates significance at p < .001
Note: Career decision-making self-efficacy as measured by the Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy Scale (Taylor
& Betz, 1983); Career certainty, as measured by the Career Certainty Scale (adapted from Osipow et al., 1976);
POK = Perceived occupational knowledge as measured by the Perceived Occupational Knowledge Scale (Pesch,
2014a).
Additional Analyses
In order to examine relations between all six variables simultaneously, a path analysis
was run using the recursive model (i.e., direct effects of each predictor variable on all subsequent
variables in the model were tested, with no reciprocal effects; Cohen et al., 2003) illustrated in
Figure 1. Table 3 presents the standardized path coefficients for the series of five steps involved
in the model predicting career certainty. The predictor variables are listed on the left side of the
table in the order they were hypothesized in the model. Criterion variables for each equation are
listed across the top, associated with the five steps. The direct effects of each predictor variable
on each criterion variable are listed under the corresponding step. Indirect effects are indicated
with arrows below the associated predictor variable. The standardized path coefficients listed in
the table represent the unique contributions of the corresponding predictor variables on the
.23*
.37* Career decisionmaking selfefficacy
Perceived
occupational
knowledge
Career
certainty
.41*
67
criterion variable, after taking into account the other predictor variables included in that
particular model. Both direct and indirect effects are reported in the table; though indirect effects
involving more than two predictor variables were not reported. Summary statistics are reported
in the table, including the proportion of variance explained by the predictor variable(s) for each
model (R
2
), the sums of squares regression (sum of the squared differences predicted and
observed values) and the mean squared errors (indicators of quality of the estimated model;
Cohen et al., 2003).
The proposed model contained one exogenous variable, career decision-making selfefficacy. The first endogenous variable was career exploration activities, followed by objective
occupational knowledge, interest congruence, perceived occupational knowledge, and career
certainty. The previous mediation hypotheses examined specific relations among these variables,
but not all of them simultaneously. Figure 7 presents the full model with the standardized path
coefficients from Table 3. While objective occupational knowledge was central to this model, it
only significantly related to interest congruence. Upon examination of the full model predicting
career certainty, results indicated 27% of the variance in career certainty could be explained by
the interplay of three variables: career decision-making self-efficacy, career exploration
activities, and perceived occupational knowledge. In general, it appears career decision-making
self-efficacy drives the model through its direct and indirect effects involving career exploration
activities and perceived occupational knowledge, while objective occupational knowledge and
interest congruence play no significant roles in predicting career certainty.
Table 3.
Standardized Path Coefficients for the Recursive Model Predicting Career Certainty (N = 316)
Criterion Variables
Step 1:
Career
Exploration
Activities
Step 2:
Objective
Occupational
Knowledge
Step 3:
Interest
Congruence
Step 4:
Perceived
Occupational
Knowledge
Step 5:
Career
Certainty
Predictor Variables
Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy (CDMSE) .42** .08 .04 .28** .36**
CDMSE → Career Exploration Activities -.02 -.01 .12** .02
CDMSE → Objective Occupational Knowledge .02 .00 -.01
CDMSE → Interest Congruence .00 .00
CDMSE → Perceived Occupational Knowledge .06*
Career Exploration Activities (CEA) -.04 -.02 .29** .04
CEA → Objective Occ. Knowledge -.02 .00 .00
CEA → Interest Congruence .00 .00
CEA → Perceived Occ. Knowledge .06*
Objective Occupational Knowledge (OOK) .20** .04 -.07
OOK → Interest Congruence .01 .02
OOK → Perceived Occ. Knowledge .01
Interest Congruence (IC) .06 .08
IC → Perceived Occ. Knowledge .01
Perceived Occupational Knowledge .22**
R
2
.18** .01 .04 .24** .27**
Sum of Squares Regression 2690.51 .18 4.44 52.20 96.44
Mean Squared Error 40.18 .11 .33 .54 .82
*Indicates significance at p < .003
**Indicates significance at p < .001
Note: All path coefficients are listed in the table. Empty cells indicate that particular path was not analyzed. Career decision-making self-efficacy as measured by
the Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy Scale (Taylor & Betz, 1983); career exploration activities as measured by the Career Exploration Checklist (Pesch,
2014b); objective occupational knowledge as measured by the Work Activity Importance Profile (adapted from National Center for O*NET Development,
2003); interest congruence as measured by the O*NET Interest Profiler (Rounds et al., 2010); perceived occupational knowledge as measured by the Perceived
Occupational Knowledge Scale (Pesch, 2014a); career certainty, as measured by the Career Certainty Scale (adapted from Osipow et al., 1976).
69
-.017
.082
.038
.199*
.041
.361*
.221*
.278* .291*
-.037
-.072
.058 .080
.035
Figure 7. Recursive path model predicting career certainty.
Note. Career decision-making self-efficacy as measured by the Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy Scale (Taylor
& Betz, 1983); career exploration activities as measured by the Career Exploration Checklist (Pesch, 2014b);
objective occupational knowledge as measured by the Work Activity Importance Profile (adapted from National
Center for O*NET Development, 2003); interest congruence as measured by the O*NET Interest Profiler (Rounds et
al., 2010); perceived occupational knowledge as measured by the Perceived Occupational Knowledge Scale (Pesch,
2014a); career certainty, as measured by the Career Certainty Scale (adapted from Osipow et al., 1976). Solid lines
indicate significant paths; dashed lines indicate non-significant paths.
*Indicates significance at p < .003
**Indicates significance at p < .001
.419*
Career decisionmaking self-efficacy
Perceived
occupational
knowledge
Interest
congruence
Career certainty
Objective
occupational
knowledge
Career
exploration
activities
70
CHAPTER FIVE
DISCUSSION
A major construct involved in the career decision-making process is occupational
knowledge. Occupational knowledge has been defined as the accuracy one has in understanding
information related to careers (Schmitt-Wilson & Welsh, 2012). Occupational knowledge has
been emphasized as an important factor in the prominent career decision-making models (Hirschi
& Lage, 2007). Following these models, one would expect increasing occupational knowledge to
lead individuals to either persist along the same career path or choose an alternative path. In turn,
greater persistence along the same career path is reflected in one’s degree of career certainty,
which is the commitment individuals have to their decisions to pursue specific careers (Daniels
et al., 2006). It is generally considered to be one of the final stages in the career decision-making
process, followed only by the planning and implementation stages (Hirschi & Lage, 2007).
Despite the centrality of career certainty in the career decision-making process, its link to
occupational knowledge had not yet been directly examined in terms of individuals’ knowledge
about their chosen careers. This deficit in the literature is in large part due to the inherent
difficulty in measuring levels of occupational knowledge individuals have regarding their chosen
careers. Therefore purpose of the present study was three-fold. First, it sought to address a hole
in the occupational knowledge literature regarding its objective measurement and its relation to
career certainty in college students. Second, since it was the first study to objectively measure
occupational knowledge of college students’ chosen careers, it sought to explore the relations
between that knowledge and four additional vocational constructs: 1) interest congruence,
defined as the degree to which a person’s interests match those of a given work environment
(Spokane & Cruza-Guet, 2005); 2) career decision-making self-efficacy, defined as one’s belief
71
that he or she is able to successfully engage in and complete tasks necessary to career decisionmaking (Betz, 2000); 3) career exploration activities participants reported, a behavioral indicator
of participants’ engagement in career exploration; and 4) perceived occupational knowledge, a
subjective self-assessment of occupational knowledge which has been frequently used in studies
as a measure of “occupational knowledge.” Third, the present study sought to examine these four
variables alongside occupational knowledge in terms of their roles in predicting career certainty.
Overall, the present study sought to gain a better understanding of the factors involved in making
career decisions in college students, with particular focus on objective occupational knowledge.
A path model predicting career certainty was used to test specific mediation hypotheses
involving occupational knowledge and the four other variables-of-interest.
Overview of Findings
Consistent with previous research, career decision-making self-efficacy had moderate
positive correlations with career certainty (Creed & Patton, 2003; Creed et al., 2005; Restubog et
al., 2010; Taylor & Popma, 1990; Tracey, 2008) and reported number of career exploration
behaviors. Career certainty additionally significantly correlated to reported number of career
exploration behaviors and perceived occupational knowledge (Westbrook, 1985), but did not
significantly correlate with interest congruence (Slaney & Slaney, 1986). Correlational findings
in the present study were similar to some previous occupational knowledge research. Objective
occupational knowledge correlated significantly, though modestly, with interest congruence
(Westbrook & Parry-Hill, 1973), providing some indication the Work Activity Importance
Profile was a valid measure of occupational knowledge. Objective occupational knowledge did
not significantly correlate with career certainty (Westbrook et al., 1996; Westbrook & Sloan,
2006), perceived occupational knowledge (Grotevant & Durrett, 1980), reported number of
72
.36*
career exploration activities, or career decision-making self-efficacy. These results suggest
occupational knowledge, as measured by the Work Activity Importance Profile, does not relate
to college students’ certainty about the careers they are pursuing, their self-estimates of
knowledge about those careers, the amount of career exploration they have engaged in, or their
beliefs about their abilities to make career decisions.
Hypotheses
Hypothesis 1 predicted objective occupational knowledge would partially mediate the
relation between career decision-making self-efficacy and career certainty. Path analysis results
indicated this hypothesis was not supported by the data. Instead, a significant relation existed
between career decision-making self-efficacy and career certainty after all other variables have
been accounted for in the model; objective occupational knowledge does not significantly relate
to either variable.
Hypothesis 2 predicted career exploration activities would partially mediate the relation
between career decision-making self-efficacy and objective occupational knowledge. Path
analysis results indicated this hypothesis was not supported by the data. Objective occupational
knowledge did not significantly relate to either variable; though career decision-making selfefficacy did have a significant direct effect on career exploration activities.
Hypothesis 3 predicted interest congruence would partially mediate the relation between
objective occupational knowledge and career certainty. Path analysis results indicated this partial
mediation hypothesis was not supported by the data. Results suggested objective occupational
knowledge and interest congruence were significantly related; though neither had a significant
relation with career certainty.
73
Finally, Hypothesis 4 predicted objective occupational knowledge would partially
mediate the relation between career decision-making self-efficacy and perceived occupational
knowledge. Path analysis results indicated this hypothesis was also not supported by the data. A
significant direct effect of career decision-making self-efficacy on perceived occupational
knowledge was found; though the indirect effect of career decision-making self-efficacy on
perceived occupational knowledge through objective occupational knowledge was not
significant.
Relation of Occupational Knowledge to Career Certainty
One flaw in the existing occupational knowledge literature is the difficulty in accurately
and adequately measuring occupational knowledge specific to careers individuals are actively
pursuing. Work Activity Importance Profiles (WAIP) based on O*NET data were introduced as
a possible solution to the pervasive measurement problem that has thus far impeded vocational
psychologists’ abilities to understand how knowledge plays a role in career decision-making.
While it remains uncertain whether the present study’s methodology validly measures true
occupational knowledge, a few specific pieces of evidence support its use in assessing this
construct. First, while its analysis required more preparation than most assessment methods, the
WAIP allowed for investigation of participants’ knowledge specific to the careers they were
actively pursuing at the time rather than their knowledge of the “working world” (e.g., Creed et
al., 2007), general clusters of careers (e.g., Job Knowledge Survey; Loesch, 1977), stereotypical
careers (e.g., Career Maturity Inventory; Crites, 1978), or their self-estimated knowledge (e.g.,
Career Development Inventory; Super et al., 1981). Second, the WAIP focused on 41 work
activities that were applicable to some degree across all careers. Participants were forced to
consider the daily experience of the careers they were pursuing rather than superficial,
74
stereotypical information they had gained. Third, convergent validity was achieved through its
relation to interest congruence. Career decision-making models (e.g., Hirschi & Lage, 2007) and
vocational interest theory (e.g., Holland, 1997) both suggest increased knowledge of the career
one is pursuing would lead one to determine whether it is a good fit. Fit, in this case, is
determined by the congruence between one’s vocational interests and the interests fulfilled by
the career he or she is pursuing. The present study found that objective occupational knowledge,
measured by the WAIP, correlated .20 with interest congruence, measured by Holland’s (1997)
theory of vocational interests and O*NET data. This finding indicated that degree of
occupational knowledge was modestly related to the degree to which participants were pursuing
careers congruent with their vocational interests. Both of these constructs have been weakly to
moderately related to job satisfaction in past research (e.g., Thomas & Bruning, 1984; Tranberg
et al., 1993). Finally, the lack of a meaningful relation of objective occupational knowledge to
perceived occupational knowledge is striking, especially because perceived occupational
knowledge had a moderate correlation to career decision-making self-efficacy. This finding
provides discriminant validity for the WAIP: participants’ commitment to career decisions
appears to be influenced in part by perceptions of their decision-making abilities and knowledge
rather than being rooted in actual knowledge. This finding is consistent with the idea that
efficacy beliefs can influence behavior (Bandura, 1977).
If one considers the WAIP to be an adequate measure of objective occupational
knowledge, the full model predicting career certainty can be discussed. The present study found
career certainty to significantly relate to career decision-making self-efficacy, reported career
exploration activities, and perceived occupational knowledge. Upon examining the direct and
indirect effects of the recursive path model predicting career certainty, the significant direct
75
effects of career decision-making self-efficacy and perceived occupational knowledge remain,
though muted. Significant indirect effects were found as well; specifically, career decisionmaking self-efficacy through perceived occupational knowledge, career decision-making selfefficacy through both career exploration activities and perceived occupational knowledge, and
career exploration activities through perceived occupational knowledge. The full model had a
medium effect on career certainty (Cohen, 1992), explaining 27.4% of the variance. Notably,
neither objective occupational knowledge nor interest congruence significantly contributed to the
prediction of career certainty, directly or indirectly. Interestingly, the zero-order relation between
career exploration activities and career certainty was almost fully mediated by perceived
occupational knowledge; though career exploration activities did have a small significant indirect
effect through perceived occupational knowledge. This result suggests actual participation in
career exploration activities partially accounts for levels of perceived occupational knowledge,
which in turn drives beliefs about career certainty. Results also suggest career decision-making
self-efficacy plays a large role in driving the model.
These results make conceptual sense if career certainty is looked at through the lens of
career decision-making self-efficacy theory (Taylor & Betz, 1983). Higher career decisionmaking self-efficacy may lead individuals to approach new or threatening situations with the
belief they will be able to cope successfully, while lower career decision-making self-efficacy
may result in avoidant behavior leading to preference for the familiar and safe instead of the new
and uncertain (Bandura, 1997; Betz, 2004; Betz & Hackett, 1981; Lent et al., 1994; Taylor &
Betz, 1983). Career exploration and making career decisions are certainly new and uncertain
situations, of which college students are the determinant of success. That level of responsibility
76
may be perceived as exciting or terrifying depending on the levels of career decision-making
self-efficacy students have achieved thus far in their lives.
What is concerning based on results from the present study is the lack of influence
objective occupational knowledge has on career certainty. It appears that, in general, college
students are making career decisions in part based on career decision-making self-efficacy and
perceived occupational knowledge; relying on beliefs of success and achievement and eschewing
any need for objective proof. Likely, this behavior will not have catastrophic consequences for
most students once they finish their education and begin working; though for some overconfident
individuals, entering the working world may be more difficult than anticipated. Their lack of
previous knowledge may become apparent and they may or may not feel satisfied with the
choices they made. This latter scenario is supported by current rates of job dissatisfaction in the
general working population, which was estimated at a striking 52.8% in 2011 (Ray & Rizzacasa,
2012). Of course, this rate included individuals working in all job levels, not just college
graduates. However, even if the dissatisfaction rate were to drop to 20% for college graduates
alone, it may signal that university programs and professionals are overlooking the need to guide
students toward informed, thoughtful career decisions.
An additional group of students also deserves some attention in light of these findings:
students who appear to be taking a cautious approach to career decision-making. While these
students may be taking a more thoughtful, guarded approach to career decisions than their
overconfident counterparts, they likely experience some of their own unique struggles.
Specifically, these cautious students may be dealing with greater current distress resulting from
worries or fears about their futures, and low confidence or self-esteem. The overconfident
students may be at greater risk of setting themselves up for future distress in the form of job
77
dissatisfaction, career changes, or potentially depressive symptoms due to the lack of personal
fulfillment provided by their jobs. In contrast, the cautious students may be experiencing similar
levels of distress currently; adding to their difficulty making a career decision. Both of these
groups should be the focus of attention from university professionals and academic major
program personnel in order to preemptively remedy potential effects of poor career decisionmaking.
Conclusions & Implications
In an attempt to improve assessment of objective occupational knowledge in college
students, the present study utilized the Work Activities Importance Profile in tandem with five
variables to predict career certainty. Importantly, objective occupational knowledge of
participants’ chosen careers significantly related to interest congruence; however, neither
variable significantly contributed to variance in career certainty. Results indicated career
decision-making self-efficacy largely drives participation in career exploration activities and
perceived occupational knowledge, and all three variables contribute to the prediction of career
certainty through direct or indirect effects. This study was the first to objectively assess college
students’ knowledge of the careers they were actively pursuing, and the first to examine that
construct along with career decision-making self-efficacy, participation in career exploration
activities, perceived occupational knowledge, interest congruence, and career certainty in a large
college student sample (N = 316).
If the results of this study are truly reflective of career decision processes being utilized
by college students, the implications would be widespread and compel universities to respond
accordingly. Results suggest college students are determining their career certainty in part based
on perceptions of career decision-making ability and perceived knowledge of chosen careers
78
instead of on objective evidence. These students should be encouraged to seek out real-world
experiences of the careers they are pursuing as a way of confirming decisions or doubts.
University officials should also be encouraging students to assess and understand their reasons
for choosing and persisting in academic major programs, and providing resources to help guide
them through this process in a thoughtful and comprehensive way. Career counseling and
exploration services should be emphasized as important aids in making good career decisions
(i.e., based on accurate and comprehensive knowledge, and interest congruence), and utilized
more often than not by students before they are allowed to commit to a degree program. These
practices would likely result in greater proportions of students graduating with majors they can
be confident will lead them to satisfying careers. The alternative, it seems, is that students may
be navigating through external pressures in the form of expectations from parents and society,
financial debt, and timeliness in degree completion, that are making them persist in somewhat
incongruent major programs or experience distress related to paralyzing indecision. Neither of
these scenarios is ideal; further research should be conducted to understand the process and
experience of college students in order to best prepare them for lives in the working world.
Limitations
The limitations in the present study are largely related to the assessment of objective
occupational knowledge. This was the first time O*NET’s work activity profiles were used in
research for the purpose of measuring participants’ levels of knowledge about careers they were
actively pursuing. While evidence for the validity of this measure was presented earlier in this
section, it remains a new method of measurement and, thus, its validity and reliability remain
speculative. Furthermore, participants’ free responses indicating which career each one was
actively pursuing had to be qualitatively matched to O*NET’s career titles. While some career
79
title matches were obvious (e.g., “accountant”), others were subject to judgment by the author
and her major professor (e.g., “FBI” became “Criminal Investigators and Special Agents”). The
O*NET also did not have data for every career indicated by participants (e.g., military officer).
Future research utilizing this assessment method may consider providing O*NET career titles for
participants to choose from rather than allowing free-responses. Additionally, the Career
Certainty Scale was adapted by the author from a previous scale (Osipow et al., 1976), and the
Perceived Occupational Knowledge Scale and the Career Exploration Checklist were created by
the author for use in the present study. Notably, however, reliability in the form of coefficient
alpha was acceptable for all these scales.
An additional important limitation is related to the present study’s sample. Participants
were recruited from a large Midwestern university, which resulted in a sample of mostly
European American students; only 19.6% of the sample identified belonging to an ethnic group
other than the majority. This limitation requires that the present study’s results be interpreted
with caution, as they may not be applicable to more ethnically-diverse samples.
Future Directions
Given the present study was the first of its kind in many respects, future researchers can
build off of its findings in various ways. Initially, further research should be done using the Work
Activity Importance Profiles in order to determine its reliability and validity. In contrast, other
forms of objective occupational knowledge could be examined that may provide more proximal
assessments of college student knowledge (e.g., testing knowledge of one’s academic major,
comparing personal work values to those fulfilled by careers-of-interest), thus providing a more
accurate picture of their occupational knowledge. Career certainty is certainly a piece of the
80
career decision-making process that could use additional research. Variables in the present study
only explained 27.4% of the variance in career certainty, leaving the majority unexplained.
Types of careers being pursued by college students could provide additional information
regarding the process of career decision-making. Prestige, aspiration levels, job availability,
social exposure, and prospective financial rewards, among others, could be influencing students
to pursue careers rather than job details, activities, and interest congruence. Overall, it appears
that the career decision process in college students is complex and in need of further research.
Vocational researchers, with the help of universities, should be focusing on guiding college
students toward thoughtful career decisions in hopes of ultimately maximizing future job
satisfaction.
Future research should also examine the present study’s findings in relation to more
diverse groups of students in terms of ethnicity and year in school. Examining students
longitudinally or an equal amount of students in their freshman through senior years would be
important in order to determine the nature of career certainty and interest congruence over time.
In addition, longitudinal research could shed light on the utility of career certainty in predicting
major at graduation or behavior after graduation, and whether students gravitate toward more
congruent careers over the course of their educations.
81
References
Achebe, C. (1982). Assessing the vocational maturity of students in the east central state of
Nigeria. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 20, 153-161.
Alvi, S. A., Khan, S. B., & Kirkwood, K. J. (1990). A comparison of carious indices of
differentiation for Holland’s model. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 36, 147-152.
Amabile, T. M., Hill, K. G., Hennessey, B. A., & Tighe, E. M. (1994). The work preference
inventory: Assessing intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations. Journal of
personality and social psychology, 66, 950-967.
Armstrong, P. I., Fouad, N. A., Rounds, J., & Hubert, L. (2010). Quantifying and interpreting
group differences in interest profiles. Journal of Career Assessment, 18, 115-132.
Arzi, H. J., & White, R. T. (2008). Change in teachers’ knowledge of subject matter: A 17-year
longitudinal study. Science Education, 2, 221-251.
Atchley, P., Hooker, E., Kroska, E., & Gilmour, A. (2012). Validation of an online orientation
seminar to improve career and major preparedness. Teaching of Psychology, 39, 146-151.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1986). The explanatory and predictive scope of self-efficacy theory. Journal of
Social and Clinical Psychology, 4, 359-373.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman.
Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1986). Differential engagement of self-reactive influences in
cognitive motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 38, 92-
113.
Berk, L. A., & Fekken, G. C. (1990). Person reliability evaluated in the context of vocational
interest assessment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 37, 7-16.
Beswick, K., Callingham, R., & Watson, J. (2012). The nature and development of middle
school mathematics teachers’ knowledge. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education,
15(2), 131-157.
Betz, N. E. (1992). Career assessment: A review of critical issues. In S. D. Brown & R. W. Lent
(Eds.), Handbook of counseling psychology (pp. 453–484). New York: Wiley.
Betz, N. E. (2000). Self-efficacy theory as a basis for career assessment. Journal of Career
Assessment, 8, 205-222.
82
Betz, N. E. (2004). Contributions of self-efficacy theory to career counseling: A personal
perspective. The Career Development Quarterly, 52, 340-353.
Betz, N. E., & Hackett, G. (1981). The relationship of career-related self-efficacy expectation to
perceived career options in college women and men. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
28, 399-410.
Betz, N. E., Hammond, M., & Multon, K. (2005). Reliability and validity of response continua
for the CDSE. Journal of Career Assessment, 13, 131-149.
Betz, N. E., Klein, K., & Taylor, K. M. (1996). Assessment of a short form of the Career
Decision making Self-efficacy Scale. Journal of Career Assessment, 4, 47-57.
Betz, N. E. & Voyten, K. K. (1997). Efficacy and outcome expectations influence career
exploration and decidedness. The Career Development Quarterly, 46, 179-189.
Blank, J. R. (1978). Jobs-Career Key: A test of occupational information. Vocational Guidance
Quarterly, 27, 6-17.
Blustein, D. L. (1997). A context-rich perspective on career exploration across life roles. Career
Development Quarterly, 45, 260-274.
Bowling, N. A., Eschleman, K. J., & Wang, Q. (2010). A meta-analytic examination of the
relationship between job satisfaction and subjective well-being. Journal of Occupational
and Organizational Psychology, 83, 915-934.
Choi, B. Y., Park, H., Yang, E., Lee, S. K., Lee, Y., & Lee, S. M. (2012). Understanding career
decision self-efficacy: A meta-analytic approach. Journal of Career Development, 39,
443-460.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation
analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Inc.
Creed, P. A., & Patton, W. (2003). Differences in career attitude and career knowledge for high
school students with and without paid work experience. International Journal for
Educational and Vocational Guidance, 3, 21-33.
Creed, P. A., Patton, W., & Hood, M. (2010). Career development and personal functioning
differences between work-bound and non-work bound students. Journal of Vocational
Behavior, 76, 37-41.
Creed, P. A., Patton, W., & Prideaux, L. (2007). Predicting change over time in career planning
and career exploration for high school students. Journal of Adolescence, 30, 377-392.
83
Creed, P. A., Prideaux, L., & Patton, W. (2005). Antecedents and consequences of career
decisional states in adolescence. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 67, 397-412.
Crites, J. O. (1973). Theory and research handbook for the Career Maturity Inventory.
Monterey, CA: CTB, McGraw Hill.
Crites, J. O. (1976). A comprehensive model of career development in early adulthood. Journal
of Vocational Behavior, 9, 105-118
Crites, J. O. (1978). Career maturity inventory. Monterey, CA: CTB/McGraw-Hill.
Cronbach, L. J., & Gleser, G. C. (1953). Assessing similarity between profiles. The
Psychological Bulletin, 50, 456-473.
Crowley, A. D. (1981). Evaluating the impact of a third-year careers education programme.
British Journal of Guidance & Counselling, 9, 207-213.
Daniels, L. M., Clifton, R. A., Perry, R. P., Mandzuk, D., & Hall, N. C. (2006). Student teachers’
competence and career certainty: The effects of career anxiety and perceived control.
Social Psychology of Education, 9, 405-423.
Dawis, R. V. (2005). The Minnesota theory of work adjustment. In Brown, S.D., & Lent, R.W.
(Eds.), Career Development and Counseling: Putting Theory and Research to Work (pp.
3-23). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
DeFleur, L. B., & Menke, B. A. (1975). Learning about the labor force: Occupational knowledge
among high school males. Sociology of Education, 48, 324-345.
Donohue, R. (2006). Person-environment congruence in relation to career change and career
persistence. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 68, 504-515.
Duffy, R. D., & Klingaman, E. A. (2009). Ethnic identity and career development among firstyear college students. Journal of Career Assessment, 17, 286-297.
Durr, M. R., & Tracey, T. J. G. (2009). Relation of person-environment fit to career certainty.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 75, 129-138.
Fouad, N. A. (1995). Career linking: An intervention to promote math and science career
awareness. Journal of Counseling & Development, 73, 527-534.
Frazier, P. A., Tix, A. P., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Testing moderator and mediator effects in
counseling psychology. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 115-134.
Gable, R. A., Tonelson, S. W., Manasi, S., Wilson, C., & Park, K. L. (2012). Importance, usage,
and preparedness to implement evidence-based practices for students with emotional
84
disabilities: A comparison of knowledge and skills of special education and general
education teachers. Education and Treatment of Children, 35, 499-519.
Gasser, C. E., Larson, L. M., & Borgen, F. H. (2007). Concurrent validity of the 2005 Strong
Interest Inventory: An examination of gender and major field of study. Journal of Career
Assessment, 15, 23-43.
Gati, I. (1986). Making career decision – A sequential elimination approach. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 33, 408-417.
Greenhaus, J. H., Hawkins, B. L., & Brenner, O. C. (1983). The impact of career exploration on
the career decision-making process. Journal of College Student Personnel, 24, 495-502.
Grotevant, H. D., & Durrett, M. E. (1980). Occupational knowledge and career development in
adolescence. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 17, 171-182.
Gunkel, M., Schlaegel, C., Langella, I. M., & Peluchette, J. V. (2010). Personality and career
decisiveness: An international empirical comparison of business students’ career
planning. Personnel Review, 39, 503-524.
Hackett, G., & Betz, N. E. (1995). Self-efficacy and career choice and development. In Hackett,
G., & Betz, N. E. (Eds.), Self-efficacy, Adaptation, and Adjustment: Theory, Research,
and Application (pp. 249-280). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Hansen, J. C. (1985). Career development inventory. Measurement and Assessment in
Counseling and Development, 17, 220-224.
Harren, V. A. (1979). A model of career decision making for college students. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 14, 119-133.
Hartman, B., Fuqua, D., & Hartman, P. (1983). The construct validity of the Career Decision
Scale administered to high school students. Vocational Guidance Quarterly, 31, 250–258.
Healy, C. C., & Mourton, D. L. (1984). The effects of an abbreviated self-directed search on the
career decision competencies of community college students. Vocational Guidance
Quarterly, 33, 55-62.
Hirschi, A. (2009). Development and criterion validity of differentiated and elevated vocational
interests in adolescence. Journal of Career Assessment, 17, 384-401.
Hirschi, A. (2011). Career-choice readiness in adolescence: Developmental trajectories and
individual differences. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 79, 340-348.
Hirschi, A., & Lage, D. (2007). The relation of secondary students’ career-choice readiness to a
six-phase model of career decision making. Journal Career Development, 34, 164-191.
85
Holland, J. L. (1971). The counselor’s guide to the Self-Directed Search. Odessa, FL:
Psychological Assessment Resources.
Holland, J. L. (1997). Making Vocational Choices: A theory of vocational personalities and
work environments (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Holland, J. L., & Gottfredson, G. D. (1975). Predictive value of psychological meaning of
vocational aspirations. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 6, 349-363.
Johnson, N., Johnson, S. C., Johnson, J., & Yates, C. (1982). Use of the vocational exploration
group with economically disadvantaged black students. Journal for Specialists in Group
Work, 7(2), 96-101.
Johnson, N., Johnson, J., & Yates, C. (1981). A 6-month follow-up on the effects of the
vocational exploration group on career maturity. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 28,
70-71.
Jones, L. K. (1989). Measuring a three-dimensional construct of career indecision among college
students: A revision of the vocational decision scale – The Career Decision Profile.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 477-486.
Jordan, T. E., & Pope, M. L. (2001). Developmental antecedents to adolescents’ occupational
knowledge: A 17-year prospective study. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 279-292.
Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing
one’s own incompetence leads to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 77, 1121–1134.
Kuder, G. F. (1944). Intermediate manual for the Kuder Preference Record. Oxford, England:
Science Research Associates.
Larson, L. M., Heppner, P. P., Ham, T., & Dugan, K. (1988). Investigating multiple subtypes of
career indecision through cluster analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35, 439-
446.
Lent, R.W., Brown, S.D., & Hackett, G. (1994). Toward a unifying social cognitive theory of
career and academic interest, choice, and performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior,
45, 79-123.
Locke, E. A., Frederick, E., Lee, C., & Bobko, P. (1984). Effect of self-efficacy, goals, and task
strategies on task performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 241-251.
Loesch, L. C. (1977). Job knowledge survey. Unpublished mimeograph, University of Florida.
Gainesville, FL.
86
Loesch, L. C., & Sampson, J. P. (1978). Job knowledge and vocational preferences. Vocational
Guidance Quarterly, 27, 55-60.
Lokan, J. (1984). Manual of the career development inventory. Melbourne: ACER.
MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence of the mediation,
confounding, and suppression effect. Prevention Science, 1, 173-181.
Miller, L., & Hayward, R. (2006). New jobs, old occupational stereotypes: Gender and jobs in
the new economy. Journal of Education and Work, 19, 67-93.
Multon, K. D. (1995) An empirical derivation of career decision subtypes in a high school
sample. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 47, 76-92.
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2010). Mplus user’s guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles: Muthén &
Muthén.
National Center for O*NET Development (2003). Summary report: Updating the Detailed Work
Activities. Alexandria, VA: Author.
Nauta, M. M. (2007). Assessing college students’ satisfaction with their academic majors.
Journal of Career Assessment, 15, 446-462.
Omvig, C. P., Tulloch, R. W., & Thomas, E. G. (1975). The effect of career education on career
maturity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 7, 265-273.
Ones, D. S., & Viswesvaran, C. (2007). A research note on the incremental validity of job
knowledge and integrity tests for predicting maximal performance. Human Performance,
20, 293-303.
Osipow, S. H. (1980). Manual for the Career Decision Scale. Columbus, OH: Marathon
Consulting & Press.
Osipow, S. H., Carney, C. G., & Barak, A. (1976). A scale of educational-vocational
undecidedness: A typological approach. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 9, 233-243.
Palumbo, M. V., Miller, C. E., Shalin, V. L., & Steele-Johnson, D. (2005). The impact of job
knowledge in the cognitive ability-performance relationship. Applied H. R. M. Research,
10(1), 13-20.
Patterson, C., Curtis, J., & Reid, A. (2008). Skills, knowledge, and attitudes expected of a newlygraduated mental health nurse in an inpatient setting. International Journal of Mental
Health Nursing, 17, 410-418.
Patton, W., & Creed, P. (2007). Occupational aspirations and expectations of Australian
adolescents. Australian Journal of Career Development, 16, 46-59.
87
Patton, W., Spooner-Lane, R., & Creed, P. (2005). Validation of the Short Form of the Career
Developmental Inventory – Australian Version with a sample of university students.
Australian Journal of Career Development, 14(3), 49-60.
Pesch, K. M. (2013a). Perceived Career Knowledge Scale. Unpublished test, Iowa State
University. Ames, Iowa.
Pesch, K. M. (2013b). Career Exploration Checklist. Unpublished test, Iowa State University.
Ames, Iowa.
Pitt, L. F., & Ramaseshan, B. (1995). Realistic job information and salesforce turnover: An
investigative study. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 10(5), 29-36.
Pumfrey, P. D., & Schofield, A. (1982). Work experience and the career maturity of fifth-form
pupils. British Journal of Guidance & Counselling, 10, 167-175.
Ray, R. L., & Rizzacasa, T. (2012). Job Satisfaction: 2012 Edition. New York, NY: The
Conference Board, Inc.
Reardon, F., Senier, J., & Lewis, J. (1972). The development and Assessment of an occupational
inventory. Journal of Educational Measurement, 9, 151-153.
Research Triangle Institute (2006). O*NET Data Collection Program. Retrieved from
https://onet.rti.org/pdf/index.cfm.
Restubog, S. L., Florentino, A. R., & Garcia, P. R. J. M. (2010). The mediating roles of career
self-efficacy and career decidedness in the relationship between contextual support and
persistence. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 77, 186-195.
Roe, A. (1956). The psychology of occupations. New York, NY: John Wiley.
Rohlfing, J. E., Nota, L., Ferrari, L., Soresi, S., & Tracey, T. J. G. (2012). Relation of
occupational knowledge to career interests and competence perceptions in Italian
children. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 81, 330-337.
Rottinghaus, P. J., Buelow, K. L., Matyja, A., & Schneider, M. R. (2012). The Career Futures
Inventory – Revised: Measuring dimensions of career adaptability. Journal of Career
Assessment, 20, 123-139.
Rottinghaus, P. J., Day, S. X., & Borgen, F. H. (2005). The Career Futures Inventory: A measure
of career-related adaptability and optimism. Journal of Career Assessment, 13, 3-24.
Rounds, J., Smith, T., Hubert, L., Lewis, P., & Rivkin, D. (1999). Development of occupational
interest profiles for O*NET. Raleigh, NC: O*NET Consortium.
88
Rounds, J., Su, R., Lewis, P., & Rivkin, D. (2010). O*NET Interest Profiler Short Form
psychometric characteristics: Summary. Raleigh, NC: National Center for O*NET
Development.
Sackett, S. A., & Hansen, J. C. (1995). Vocational outcomes of college freshmen with flat
profiles on the strong interest inventory. Measurement and Assessment in Counseling and
Development, 28(1), 9-24.
Sadeghi, A., Baghban, I., Bahrami, F., Ahmadi, A., & Creed, P. (2011). Validation of the short
form of the career development inventory with an Iranian high school sample.
International Journal for Educational and Vocational Guidance, 11, 29-38.
Salomon, G. (1984). Television is “easy” and print is “tough”: The differential investment of
mental effort in learning as a function of perceptions and attributions. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 76, 647-658.
Sampson, J. P., Jr., Reardon, R. C., Peterson, G. W., & Lenz, J. G. (2004). Career counseling
and services: A cognitive information processing approach. Pacific Grove, CA:
Brooks/Cole.
Sampson, J. P., & Loesch, L. C. (1981). Relationships among work values and job knowledge.
Vocational Guidance Quarterly, 29, 229-235.
Schmitt-Wilson, S., & Welsh, M. C. (2012). Vocational knowledge in rural children: A study of
individual differences and predictors of occupational aspirations and expectations.
Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 862-867.
Slaney, R. B., & Slaney, F. M. (1986). Relationship of expressed and inventoried vocational
interests of female career counseling clients. The Career Development Quarterly, 35(1),
24-33.
Spokane, A. R., & Cruz-Guet, M. C. (2005). Holland’s theory of vocational personalities in
work environments. In Brown, S.D., & Lent, R.W. (Eds.), Career Development and
Counseling: Putting Theory and Research to Work (pp. 24-41). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley
& Sons, Inc.
Su, R., Rounds, J., & Armstrong, P. I. (2009). Men and things, women and people: A metaanalysis of sex differences in interests. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 859-884.
Super, D. E. (1983). Assessment in career guidance: Toward truly developmental counseling.
The Personnel and Guidance Journal, 61, 555-562.
Super, D. E., Thompson, A. S., Lindeman, R. H., Jordaan, J. P., & Myers, R. A. (1981). The
career development inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
89
Taylor, M. S. (1985). The roles of occupational knowledge and vocational self-concept
crystallization in students’ school-to-work transition. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
32, 539-550.
Taylor, K. M., & Betz, N. E. (1983). Applications of self-efficacy theory to the understanding
and treatment of career indecision. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 22, 63-81.
Taylor, K. M., & Popma, J. (1990). An examination of the relationships among career decisionmaking self-efficacy, career salience, locus of control, and vocational indecision. Journal
of Vocational Behavior, 37, 17-31.
Theophilides, C., Terenzini, P. T., & Lorang, W. (1984). Relation between freshman-year
experience and perceived importance of four major educational goals. Research in
Higher Education, 20, 235-252.
Thomas, R. G., & Bruning, C. R. (1984). Cognitive dissonance as a mechanism in vocational
decision processes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 24, 264-278.
Tracey, T. J. G. (2002). Personal Globe Inventory: Measurement of the spherical model of
interests and competence beliefs. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 113–172.
Tracey, T. J. G. (2008). Adherence to RIASEC structure as a key career decision construct.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55(2), 146-157.
Tracey, T. J. G. (2010). Relation of interest and self-efficacy occupational congruence and career
choice certainty. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76, 441-447.
Tracey, T. J. G., & Darcy, M. (2002). An idiothetic examination of vocational interests and their
relation to career decidedness. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 49(4), 420-427.
Tranberg, M., Slane, S., & Ekeberg, S. E. (1993). The relation between interest congruence and
satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 42, 253-264.
Tsabari, O., Tziner, A., & Meir, E. I. (2005). Updated meta-analysis on the relationship between
congruence and satisfaction. Journal Career Assessment, 13, 216-232.
Van Iddekinge, C. H., Putka, D. J., & Campbell, J. P. (2011). Reconsidering vocational interests
for personnel selection: The validity of an interest-based selection test in relation to job
knowledge, job performance, and continuance intentions. Journal of Applied Psychology,
96, 13-33.
Wall, J. E., & Baker, H. E. (1997). The interest-finder: Evidence of validity. Journal of Career
Assessment, 5, 255-273.
Walls, R. T. (2000). Vocational cognition: Accuracy of 3rd
-, 6th
-, 9th
-, and 12th
-grade students.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56, 137-144.
90
Ware, M. E., & Pogge, D. L. (1980). Concomitants of certainty in career-related choices.
Vocational Guidance Quarterly, 28, 322-327.
Watson, M. B., & Foxcroft, C. D. (1997). Occupational aspirations of black south African
adolescents. Psychological Reports, 80, 627-635.
Westbrook, B. W. (1976). The relationship between vocational maturity and appropriateness of
vocational choices of ninth-grade pupils. Measurement and Assessment in Guidance, 9(2),
75-80.
Westbrook, B. W. (1985). Predictive and construct validity of six experimental measures of
career maturity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 27, 338-355.
Westbrook, B. W. (1997). Career Planning Inventory. Unpublished test, North Carolina State
University. Raleigh, North Carolina.
Westbrook, B. W., Elrod, T., & Wynne, C. (1996). Career assessment and the Cognitive
Vocational Maturity Test. Journal of Career Assessment, 4, 139-170.
Westbrook, B. W., & Parry-Hill, J. W. (1973). The measurement of cognitive vocational
maturity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 3, 239-252.
Westbrook, B., & Sloan, S. (2006). Construct validity and predictive validity of career planning
inventory. Journal of Career Assessment, 14, 385-404.
Williams, D. F. (2011). The impact of career workshops on freshman college students at-risk for
dropout: An action research study. Journal of College Student Retention: Research,
Theory & Practice, 13, 37-62.
Yanico, B. J., & Hardin, S. I. (1986). College students’ self-estimated and actual knowledge of
gender traditional and nontraditional occupations: A replication and extension. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 28, 229-240.
Yanico, B. J., & Mihlbauer, T. C. (1983). Students’ self-estimated and actual knowledge of
gender traditional and nontraditional occupations. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 22,
278-287.
Zener, T. B., & Schnuelle, L. (1976). Effects of the Self-Directed Search on high school
students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 23, 353-359.
91
APPENDIX A
Demographics Questionnaire
Age: _______
Gender: Male
Female
Other
Ethnicity: Asian American/Pacific Islander
Arab American
Caucasian/White
Native American
Black/African American
Latino/a American
Biracial ______________
International Student
Other: _____________
Year in school: First
Second
Third
Fourth
Fifth
Sixth
Graduate Student
Have you declared your major? YES / NO
If YES, what is it? _____________
If NO, what major are you considering? _____________
What career are you currently considering pursuing? _____________
What is the education level required for that career?
High school degree
Bachelor’s degree
Master’s degree
Doctoral degree (e.g., PhD, MD, JD)
92
APPENDIX B
Work Activity Importance Profile: O*NET Detailed Work Activities
(National Center for O*NET Development, 2003)
Not Important Somewhat
Important Impotant Very Important Extremely
Important
1 2 3 4 5
1 Getting Information: Observing, receiving, and otherwise obtaining information from all
relevant sources.
2 Identifying Objects, Actions, and Events: Identifying information by categorizing,
estimating, recognizing differences or similarities, and detecting changes in circumstances or
events.
3 Monitoring Processes, Materials, or Surroundings: Monitoring and reviewing information
from materials, events, or the environment, to detect or assess problems.
4 Inspecting Equipment, Structures, or Materials: Inspecting equipment, structures, or
materials to identify the cause of errors or other problems or defects.
5 Estimating the Quantifiable Characteristics of Products, Events, or Information:
Estimating sizes, distances, and quantities; or determining time, costs, resources, or materials
needed to perform activity.
6 Judging the Qualities of Objects, Services, or People: Assessing the value, importance, or
quality of things or people.
7 Evaluating Information to Determine Compliance with Standards: Using relevant
information and individual judgment to determine whether events or processes comply with
laws, regulations, or standards
8 Processing Information: Compiling, coding, categorizing, calculating, tabulating, auditing,
or verifying information or data.
9 Analyzing Data or Information: Identifying the underlying principles, reasons, or facts of
information by breaking down information or data into separate parts.
10 Making Decisions and Solving Problems: Analyzing information and evaluating results to
choose the best solution and solve problems.
11 Thinking Creatively: Developing, designing, or creating new applications, ideas,
relationships, systems, or products, including artistic contributions.
12 Updating and Using Relevant Knowledge: Keeping up-to-date technically and applying
new knowledge to your job.
93
13 Developing Objectives and Strategies: Establishing long-range objectives and specifying
the strategies and actions to achieve them.
14 Scheduling Work and Activities: Scheduling events, programs, and activities, as well as the
work of others.
15 Organizing, Planning, and Prioritizing Work: Developing specific goals and plans to
prioritize, organize, and accomplish your work.
16 Performing General Physical Activities: Performing physical activities that require
considerable use of your arms and legs and moving your whole body, such as climbing,
lifting, balancing, walking and handling of materials.
17 Handling and Moving Objects: Using hands and arms in handling, installing, positioning,
moving materials, and manipulating things.
18 Controlling Machines and Processes: Using either control mechanisms or direct physical
activity to operate machines or processes (not including computers or vehicles).
19 Working with Computers: Using computers and computer systems (including hardware and
software) to program, write software, set up functions, enter data, or process information.
20 Operating Vehicles, Mechanized Devices, or Equipment: Running, maneuvering,
navigating, or driving vehicles or mechanized equipment, such as forklifts, passenger
vehicles, aircraft, or water craft
21 Drafting, Laying Out, and Specifying Technical Devices, Parts, and Equipment:
Providing documentation, detailed instructions, drawings, or specifications to tell others
about how devices, parts, e or structures are to be fabricated, constructed, assembled,
modified, maintained, or used.
22 Repairing and Maintaining Mechanical Equipment: Servicing, repairing, adjusting, and
testing machines, devices, moving parts, and equipment that operate primarily on the basis of
mechanical (not electronic) principles.
23 Repairing and Maintaining Electronic Equipment: Servicing, repairing, calibrating,
regulating, fine-tuning, or testing machines, devices, and equipment that operate primarily on
the basis of elect electronic (not mechanical) principles.
24 Documenting/Recording Information: Entering, transcribing, recording, storing, or
maintaining information in written or electronic/magnetic form.
25 Interpreting the Meaning of Information for Others: Translating or explaining what
information means and how it can be used.
94
26 Communicating with Supervisors, Peers, or Subordinates: Providing information to
supervisors, coworkers, and subordinates by telephone, in written form, e-mail, or in person.
27 Communicating with People Outside the Organization: Communicating with people
outside the organization, representing the organization to customers, the public, government,
and other external source information can be exchanged in person, in writing, or by telephone
or e-mail.
28 Establishing and Maintaining Interpersonal Relationships: Developing constructive and
cooperative working relationships with others, and maintaining them over time.
29 Helping and Caring for Others: Providing personal Helpance, medical attention,
emotional support, or other personal care to others such as coworkers, customers, or patients.
30 Selling or Influencing Others: Convincing others to buy merchandise/goods or to otherwise
change their minds or actions.
31 Resolving Conflicts and Negotiating with Others: Handling complaints, settling disputes,
and resolving grievances and conflicts, or otherwise negotiating with others.
32 Performing for or Working Directly with the Public: Performing for people or dealing
directly with the public. This includes serving customers in restaurants and stores, and
receiving clients or guests.
33 Coordinating the Work and Activities of Others: Getting members of a group to work
together to accomplish tasks.
34 Developing and Building Teams: Encouraging and building mutual trust, respect, and
cooperation among team members.
35 Training and Teaching Others: Identifying the educational needs of others, developing
formal educational or training programs or classes, and teaching or instructing others.
36 Guiding, Directing, and Motivating Subordinates: Providing guidance and direction to
subordinates, including setting performance standards and monitoring performance.
37 Coaching and Developing Others: Identifying the developmental needs of others and
coaching, mentoring, or otherwise helping others to improve their knowledge or skills.
38 Providing Consultation and Advice to Others: Providing guidance and expert advice to
management or other groups on technical, systems-, or process-related topics.
39 Performing Administrative Activities: Performing day-to-day administrative tasks such as
maintaining information files and processing paperwork.
95
40 Staffing Organizational Units: Recruiting, interviewing, selecting, hiring, and promoting
employees in an organization.
41 Monitoring and Controlling Resources: Monitoring and controlling resources and
overseeing the spending of money.
What career did you have in mind while you were rating the importance of those activities?
__________________
96
APPENDIX C
Occupation Work Activity Importance Profile Example:
The top six work activities for accounting
97
APPENDIX D
Perceived Occupational Knowledge Scale
(Pesch, 2014a)
Please respond to the following statements about the career you listed earlier in the survey.
Strongly
disagree Disagree Somewhat
disagree
Somewhat
agree
Agree Strongly
Agree
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. I have a good understanding of the daily activities that will be involved in this job.
2. I do not know what I will be doing on a daily basis in this job.
3. I know the types of work activities I will be required to do in this job.
98
APPENDIX E
Career Certainty Scale
(Adapted from Osipow, Carney, & Barak, 1976)
Please respond to the following statements about the career you listed earlier in the survey.
Strongly
disagree Disagree Somewhat
disagree
Somewhat
agree
Agree Strongly
Agree
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. I am certain that this is the career I want when I finish my education.
2. I definitely want to pursue this career.
3. I have decided this is the career I am most interested in pursuing.
99
APPENDIX F
Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale
(Taylor & Betz, 1983)
INSTRUCTIONS: For each statement below, please read carefully and indicate how much
confidence you have that you could accomplish each of these tasks by marking your answer
according to the following 5-point continuum. Mark your answer by filling in the correct circle
on the answer sheet.
NO CONFIDENCE
AT ALL
VERY LITTLE
CONFIDENCE
MODERATE
CONFIDENCE
MUCH
CONFIDENCE
COMPLETE
CONFIDENCE
1 2 3 4 5
HOW MUCH CONFIDENCE DO YOU HAVE THAT YOU COULD:
1. List several majors that you are interested in.
2. Use the internet to find information about occupations that interest you.
3. Select one major from a list of potential majors you are considering.
4. Make a plan of your goals for the next five years.
5. Determine the steps to take if you are having academic trouble with an aspect of your
chosen major.
6. Accurately assess your abilities.
7. Find information about companies who employ people with college majors in English.
8. Select one occupation from a list of potential occupations you are considering.
9. Determine the steps you need to take to successfully complete your chosen major.
10. Persistently work at your major or career goal even when you get frustrated.
11. List several occupations that you are interested in.
12. Find information about educational programs in engineering.
13. Choose a career that will fit your preferred lifestyle.
14. Prepare a good resume.
15. Change majors if you did not like your first choice.
16. Determine what your ideal job would be.
17. Talk to a faculty member in a department you are considering for a major.
18. Make a career decision and then not worry about whether it was right or wrong.
19. Get letters of recommendation from your professors.
20. Change occupations if you are not satisfied with the one you enter.
21. Decide what you value most in an occupation.
22. Ask a faculty member about graduate schools and job opportunities in your major.
23. Choose a major or career that your parents do not approve of.
24. Get involved in a work experience relevant to your future goals.
25. Resist attempts of parents or friends to push you into a career or major you believe is
beyond your abilities.
26. Figure out whether you have the ability to successfully take math courses.
27. Describe the job duties of the career/occupation you would like to pursue.
100
28. Choose a career in which most workers are the opposite sex.
29. Find and use the Placement Office on campus.
30. Move to another city to get the kind of job you really would like.
31. Determine the academic subject you have the most ability in.
32. Find out the employment trends for an occupation in the next decade.
33. Choose a major or career that will fit your interests.
34. Decide whether or not you will need to attend graduate or professional school to achieve
your career goals.
35. Apply again to graduate school after being rejected the first time.
36. Determine whether you would rather work primarily with people or with information.
37. Find out about the average yearly earnings of people in an occupation.
38. Choose a major or career that will suit your abilities.
39. Plan course work outside of your major that will help you in your future career.
40. Identify some reasonable major or career alternatives if you are unable to get your first
choice.
41. Figure out what you are and are not ready to sacrifice to achieve your career goals.
42. Talk with a person already employed in the field you are interested in.
43. Choose the best major for you even if it took longer to finish your college degree.
44. Identify employers, firms, institutions relevant to your career possibilities.
45. Go back to school to get a graduate degree after being out of school 5-10 years.
46. Define the type of lifestyle you would like to live.
47. Find information about graduate or professional schools.
48. Choose the major you want even though the job market is declining with opportunities in
this field.
49. Successfully manage the job interview process.
50. Come up with a strategy to deal with flunking out of college.
101
APPENDIX G
Career Exploration Checklist
(Pesch, 2014b)
Please indicate whether or not you have used the following methods to gain information about
the career you chose for this survey.
Never Once More than
once
0 1 2
1. Searched the internet to learn about this career
2. Read about the career somewhere other than the internet
3. Talked with a family member who is employed in this career
4. Talked with a friend who is pursuing this career
5. Talked with a professor or graduate student about this career
6. Talked with an academic advisor about this career
7. Talked with someone who is employed in the career about the career
8. Did an informational interview with someone about this career
9. Job shadowed someone who is employed in the career
10. Did an internship or job related to the career
11. Did an externship with someone related to this career
12. Did volunteering related to the career
13. Attended a career or job fair
14. Attended a presentation about this career or this career field
15. Attended a meeting sponsored by a student organization related to this career
16. Took a career course related to this career
17. Talked to a career counselor about options
102
APPENDIX H
O*NET Interest Profiler – Short Form
(Rounds, Su, Lewis, & Rivkin, 2010)
Below are 60 questions about work activities that some people do on their jobs. Please read each
question carefully and decide how you would feel about doing each type of work:
Strongly
dislike
Strongly
Like
0 1 2 3 4
Realistic Items
1. Build kitchen cabinets
18. Lay brick or tile
19. Repair household appliances
20. Raise fish in a fish hatchery
21. Assemble electronic parts
22. Drive a truck to deliver packages to offices and homes
23. Test the quality of parts before shipment
24. Repair and install locks
25. Set up and operate machines to make products
26. Put out forest fires
Investigative Items
1. Develop a new medicine
2. Study ways to reduce water pollution
3. Conduct chemical experiments
4. Study the movement of planets
5. Examine blood samples using a microscope
6. Investigate the cause of a fire
7. Develop a way to better predict the weather
8. Work in a biology lab
9. Invent a replacement for sugar
10. Do laboratory tests to identify diseases
Artistic Items
1. Write books or plays
2. Play a musical instrument
3. Compose or arrange music
4. Draw pictures
5. Create special effects for movies
6. Paint sets for plays
7. Write scripts for movies or television shows
8. Perform jazz or tap dance
9. Sing in a band
103
10. Edit movies
Social Items
1. Teach an individual an exercise routine
2. Help people with personal or emotional problems
3. Give career guidance to people
4. Perform rehabilitation therapy
5. Do volunteer work at a non-profit organization
6. Teach children how to play sports
7. Teach sign language to people with hearing disabilities
8. Help conduct a group therapy session
9. Take care of children at a day-care center
10. Teach a high-school class
Enterprising Items
1. Buy and sell stocks and bonds
2. Manage a retail store
3. Operate a beauty salon or barber shop
4. Manage a department within a large company
5. Start your own business
6. Negotiate business contracts
7. Represent a client in a lawsuit
8. Market a new line of clothing
9. Sell merchandise at a department store
10. Manage a clothing store
Conventional Items
1. Develop a spreadsheet using computer software
2. Proofread records or forms
3. Load computer software into a large computer network
4. Operate a calculator
5. Keep shipping and receiving records
6. Calculate the wages of employees
7. Inventory supplies using a hand-held computer
8. Record rent payments
9. Keep inventory records
10. Stamp, sort, and distribute mail for an organization

Order | Check Discount

Tags: APA citation format assignment, assignment writers Australia college student, Best nursing case study writing services, buy essay USA, Case Study Nursing Writing Services for Care Plans, Free nursing case study writing services

Assignment Help For You!

Special Offer! Get 20-30% Off on Every Order!

Why Seek Our Custom Writing Services

Every Student Wants Quality and That’s What We Deliver

Graduate Essay Writers

Only the finest writers are selected to be a part of our team, with each possessing specialized knowledge in specific subjects and a background in academic writing..

Affordable Prices

We balance affordability with exceptional writing standards by offering student-friendly prices that are competitive and reasonable compared to other writing services.

100% Plagiarism-Free

We write all our papers from scratch thus 0% similarity index. We scan every final draft before submitting it to a customer.

How it works

When you opt to place an order with Nursing StudyBay, here is what happens:

Fill the Order Form

You will complete our order form, filling in all of the fields and giving us as much instructions detail as possible.

Assignment of Writer

We assess your order and pair it with a custom writer who possesses the specific qualifications for that subject. They then start the research/write from scratch.

Order in Progress and Delivery

You and the assigned writer have direct communication throughout the process. Upon receiving the final draft, you can either approve it or request revisions.

Giving us Feedback (and other options)

We seek to understand your experience. You can also peruse testimonials from other clients. From several options, you can select your preferred writer.

Expert paper writers are just a few clicks away

Place an order in 3 easy steps. Takes less than 5 mins.

Calculate the price of your order

You will get a personal manager and a discount.
We'll send you the first draft for approval by at
Total price:
$0.00